Abdulhadi E., Ireland D., Simpson R., Morrow S.A., Le E., Donkers S., Knox K.B. on behalf of the MSBEST Team. (2022). Cognitive Impairment: Non-pharmacological Rehabilitation Interventions. Multiple Sclerosis Best Evidence-Based Strategies and Treatment/Therapies for Rehabilitation. Version 1.0: p 1-219.
Podcast Summary
Author Disclosures
Dr. Sarah Donkers has received research funding from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Multiple Sclerosis Society of Canada, Branch Out Neurological Foundation, Praxis Spinal Cord Institute, Craig Neilson Foundation, Saskatchewan Health Research Foundation, Saskatchewan Centre for Patient-Oriented Research, College of Medicine University of Saskatchewan, Saskatchewan Ministry of Health, Heart and Stroke Saskatchewan, Spinal Cord Injury Saskatchewan and Saskatoon City Hospital Foundation. She has received investigator-initiated research funds from Helius Medical Technologies, and program funding from the Multiple Sclerosis Society of Canada for the NeuroSask Active and Connected Program.
Dr. Katherine Knox has received research funding from the Multiple Sclerosis Society of Canada, Saskatchewan Health Research Foundation, Saskatchewan Centre for Patient-oriented Research, Saskatchewan Ministry of Health Drug Plan and Extended Benefits Branch, College of Medicine University of Saskatchewan, and Saskatoon City Hospital foundation. She is the director for the Saskatchewan MS Drugs Research Program Quality of Life and Health Outcomes Study, is a member of the Saskatchewan MS Drugs Program Panel, was involved in a Roche sponsored clinical trial as a site investigator and received program funding from the Multiple Sclerosis Society of Canada for the NeuroSask Active and Connected Program.
Dr. Sarah Morrow has served on advisory boards for Biogen Idec, Celgene, EMD Serono, Novartis, Roche, Sanofi Genzyme, and Teva Neurosciences. She has received investigator-initiated grant funds from Biogen Idec, Novartis, Roche, and Sanofi Genzyme and has acted as site primary investigator for multicenter trials funded by AbbVie, Celgene, EMD Serono, Novartis, Roche, and Sanofi Genzyme. She has received research funding from the Multiple Sclerosis Society of Canada, National Multiple Sclerosis Society, and Canadian Institutes of Health Research.
Dr. Robert Simpson has received funding from the Canadian Institutes for Health Research for an integrated knowledge translation grant studying mindfulness for people with multiple sclerosis.
Eman Abdulhadi, Devon Ireland and Dr. Emma Le have no disclosures.
Lay Summary of Evidence
Interventions favouring a benefit for one or more cognitive outcomes
Rehabilitation approaches targeting memory improve memory in persons with MS with minimum to moderate cognitive impairment compared to no treatment.
Restitution approaches may increase self-reported stress levels compared to compensatory approaches.
Non-specific or multi-modal rehabilitation approaches delivered individually, in a group, or remotely may improve memory in persons with MS compared to no treatment.
The Modified Story Memory Technique improves verbal learning and memory but does not improve other forms of memory in persons with MS.
Rehabilitation approaches targeting executive function improve executive function outcomes in persons with MS with minimum cognitive impairment
Computer cognitive training in memory improves memory in persons with MS with mild cognitive impairment compared to no treatment
Computer cognitive training in processing speed improves processing speed in persons with MS with mild cognitive impairment compared to no treatment
Computer cognitive training in executive function improves executive function in persons with MS with mild cognitive impairment compared to no treatment
Computer cognitive training in attention may improve attention in persons with MS with mild cognitive impairment compared to no treatment
Nintendo's Braining Training video games may improve executive function and information processing speed, and the Space Fortress video game may improve spatial memory and visuospatial memory in persons with MS.
Cognitive rehabilitation carried out in a virtual reality environment may improve information processing and memory in persons with MS.
Strobic visual training may improve processing speed, but not other cognitive domains in persons with MS.
Neurofeedback training may improve long-term memory and executive function in persons with MS.
Spaced learning improves memory more than mass learning.
Retrieval practice learning improves memory more than spaced or mass learning in persons with MS with mild or severe cognitive impairment at baseline
Preliminary evidence suggests that cue salience may improve prospective memory in persons with MS.
Selective reminding tasks may improve memory in persons with MS.
Teaching the Self-Generation Technique may improve recall on memory tasks where the technique is applied.
Mental visual imagery training may improve memory in patients with relapsing remitting MS on an autobiographical memory interview assessment; other objective memory and cognitive outcomes were not reported.
Preliminary evidence supports that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may improve attention and verbal skills in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence supports that meditation may improve information processing speed in persons with relapsing-remitting MS.
Preliminary evidence supports that psychotherapy may improve memory but not attention in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence suggests that psychotherapy may improve auditory information processing speed but not visual information processing speed in persons with MS.
Music therapy may be beneficial for improving memory in persons with MS.
There is preliminary evidence that action observation training added to an upper limb rehabilitation program may improve auditory processing speed in persons with MS.
An exercise program with a cooling garment may improve verbal fluency in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence suggests that team-based artistic therapy may improve visual information processing speed and memory but may not improve auditory information processing speed in persons with relapsing-remitting MS.
Preliminary evidence suggests that a modified paleolithic diet combined with electrical stimulation, exercise, and stress management may improve executive functioning in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence supports that running may improve spatial memory but not verbal learning and memory or information processing speed in persons with MS.
High intensity aerobic training may improve verbal memory compared to moderate intensity aerobic training but may not improve cognition in other cognitive domains.
Balance training coupled with dual task training may improve general cognition and executive function compared to no intervention in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence supports that Pilates may improve information processing speed and memory for persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence supports that yoga may improve attention in persons with MS.
Functional electrical stimulation cycling may improve visual processing speed compared to passive cycling in persons with MS with mobility impairments.
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex may improve executive function when combined with cognitive training tasks.
Interventions with inconclusive or conflicting findings
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves verbal language skills in persons with MS with minimal cognitive impairment compared to no treatment.
There is conflicting evidence whether the combination of computer based cognitive rehabilitation with compensatory rehabilitation approaches provides added benefit for improving attention, information processing speed, executive function, spatial skills, verbal language skills or memory in persons with MS.
Compensatory approaches targeting memory in persons with MS may not be superior to restitution approaches, self-management coaching, or access to MS occupational therapy and nursing services for improving memory.
Non-specific cognitive rehabilitation approaches may not improve outcomes in other cognitive domains besides memory compared to no treatment.
There is conflicting evidence whether mindfulness-based cognitive therapies improve memory in persons with MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether meditation improves memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether music mnemonics improves memory in persons with MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether cooling garments improve information processing in persons with MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether cycling improves cognition in persons with MS, with positive results for improving memory in persons with progressive MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether walking programs improve information processing speed or executive function in persons with MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether yoga improves executive function in persons with MS.
Social Cognitive Education combined with exercise may improve information processing speed, but not more than Attention Control Education combined with exercise.
Interventions with no observed benefit on cognitive outcomes
The Space Fortress video game may not improve verbal learning and memory in persons with MS.
Robot-assisted gait training in a virtual reality environment may not improve information processing speed, memory, or verbal language skills more than standard robot-assisted gait training.
Robotic-assisted gait training may not improve cognitive impairment more than gait training alone in persons with MS.
Cognitive Occupation-Based Programme for People with Multiple Sclerosis (COB-MS) may not improve processing speed or executive function, but self-reported performance on ADLs, IADLs and occupational competence may improve.
Preliminary evidence suggests that cooling below the resting normal temperature may worsen memory in persons with MS.
Dual Task Training combined with gait training may not improve attention, memory, or information processing speed more than gait training alone in persons with MS.
Dual Task Training may not improve executive function more than strength training in persons with MS.
Aerobic and strength training combined may not improve information processing speed, attention, or memory after short- term follow up in MS. Long-term effects and response heterogeneity warrant further study.
Preliminary evidence from small studies supports that circuit training may not improve memory, verbal fluency, or processing speed more than relaxation exercises in persons with MS
Walking Programs may not improve verbal learning and memory in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence from small studies supports that stepping exercises may not improve cognitive outcomes in persons with MS compared to usual activity or light physical activity.
Prelimnary evidence from small studies supports that high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation may not improve working memory in persons with MS.
Transcranial random noise stimulation may not improve attention in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence supports that non-invasive tongue stimulation may not improve memory, executive function, or information processing speed in persons with MS.
Colour Coding
Abbreviations
ACT | Adaptive Cognitive Training |
AI-EFT | Autobiographical Interview for Episodic Future Thought |
AM | Autobiographical Memory |
AMI | Autobiographical Memory Interview |
AQ | Awareness Questionnaire |
AVLT | Auditory Verbal Learning Test |
BDI | Beck Depression Index |
CBT | Cognitive Behavioural Therapy |
CCR | Conventional Cognitive Rehab |
CHART-R | Craig Handicap Assessment and Rating Technique-Revised |
CI | Cognitive Impairment |
COB-MS | Cognitive Occupation-Based Programme for People with Multiple Sclerosis |
COPM | Canadian occupational performance measure |
CR | Cognitive Rehabilitation |
CST | Cognitive Screening Test |
CSI | Cognitive Stability Index |
DBT | Dialectical Behaviour Therapy |
DCAQ | Daily Cognitive Activities Questionnaire |
DKBT | Dr. Kawashima's Brain Training video game |
EMQ | Everyday Memory Questionnaire |
FAMS | Functional Assessment of MS |
GAS | Goal Attainment Scale |
GE | Generation Effect Task (GE): recall and recognition of generated or provided stimuli |
HC | Healthy Control |
HVT | Hybrid-Variable priority Training |
MBCT | Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy |
MBI | Mindfulness-based interventions |
MBSR | Mindfulness-based stress reduction |
MBT | Mindfulness-Based Training |
MFIS | Modified Fatigue Impact Scale |
MFQ | Memory Functioning Questionnaire |
MR | Massed Restudy |
MS | Multiple Sclerosis |
MS-MILD | MS-Mildly Impaired |
MS-MOD | MS-Moderately Impaired |
MS-UN | MS-Unimpaired |
mSMT | modified Story Memory Technique |
MSNQ | Multiple Sclerosis Neuropsychological Questionnaire |
MVI | Mental Visual Imagery |
NMT | neurologic music therapy |
OSA-DLS | Occupational Self-Asessment-Daily Living Scales |
PCT | Prospective Controlled Trial |
PS-TTC, -RT | Preference shifting (PS): trials to criterion (TTC), reaction time (RT) |
PSAI | Pythagorean Self-Awareness Intervention |
PwMS | Persons with Multiple Sclerosis |
RCT | Randomized Controlled Trial |
Self-GEN | Self-generation learning program |
SR | Spaced Restudy |
ST | Spaced Testing |
STEM | Strategy-based Training to Enhance Memory |
SMT | Story Memory Technique |
SVT | Stroboscopic Visual Training |
TBI | Traumatic Brain Injury |
VPA | Verbal Paired Associates |
WRAT-3 | Wide Range Achievement Test-3 (WRAT-3) reading subtest |
WTAR | Wechsler Test of Adult Reading |
1.0 Introduction
Non-pharmacological approaches addressing cognitive impairment (CI) in MS include a large variety of interventions. In persons with MS (PwMS), there is individual variability in the severity of CI and the cognitive domains affected. Over the disease course, most PwMS experience a change in cognitive function, with processing speed being the most predominantly affected (Van Schependom et al. 2015). Objective loss of grey matter and functional neural network changes are associated with CI in MS. Functional network alterations may also occur with cognitive training in PwMS (Bonavita et al. 2015; A. Ernst et al. 2018; O. Boukrina et al. 2019). CI negatively affects the quality of life of PwMS and caregivers (Labiano-Fontcuberta et al. 2014), and is associated with increased risk for future institutionalized care (Thorpe et al. 2015).
In the management of dementia, stroke, and acquired brain injury, non-pharmacological approaches addressing CI are routine care. Cognitive rehabilitation strategies are divided broadly into compensatory (i.e., external memory aids) or restorative (i.e., re-organization of information and internal encoding for enhanced retrieval). Traumatic brain injury involves different mechanisms of injury in comparison to MS, with some common molecular pathways (Macrez et al. 2016). Traumatic Brain Injury and MS both may affect younger adults where the potential for neuroplasticity may be greater in comparison to older adults. International guidelines by INCOG for cognitive rehabilitation following traumatic brain injury provide guiding principles for clinicians (Bayley et al. 2014). Cognitive rehabilitation may include restorative and compensatory strategies, caregiver training, functional adaptation and environmental manipulation, and education about the consequences of CI. Rehabilitation should be tailored to the “patient's neuropsychological profile, including considering premorbid cognitive characteristics and goals for life activities and participation” (Bayley et al. 2014 p.301).
Guidelines from The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) for the management of MS and Consensus recommendations for CI in MS both help to raise awareness about the existence of CI in MS. The Consensus recommendations are endorsed by the Consortium of Multiple Sclerosis Centers and the International Multiple Sclerosis Cognition Society (Multiple Sclerosis in Adults: Management 2019; R. Kalb et al. 2018), and emphasize screening, assessment, and referral to a specialist for remediation management (i.e., a neuropsychologist, speech language therapist, or occupational therapist). However, the NICE guidelines and the Consensus recommendations do not provide specific advice about the selection of interventions most appropriate for PwMS. Similar to acquired brain injury, the success of an intervention may depend in part on individualizing the approach to the person's neuropsychological profile and goals of treatment. For example, if insight is markedly impaired, caregiver training may be appropriate—yet there is limited research on how to best support caregivers in the management of CI in MS (Rajachandrakumar and Finlayson 2021; Clare et al. 2019).
In research settings, there may be recruitment bias towards the inclusion of more activated PwMS. Activation, motivation, and practice are key ingredients for learning. In clinical practice, mood and fatigue symptoms and cognitive fatigability (the inability to maintain performance throughout a sustained cognitive task (Walker, Berard, and Walker 2021)) are common. For these reasons, interventions demanding sustained attention may be less feasible for PwMS. The 2018 Consensus recommendations (R. Kalb et al. 2018) list promising interventions for CI from positive MS pivotal trials, including remediation techniques such as spaced learning and retrieval practice (Sumowski, Chiaravalloti, and DeLuca 2010), supervised computer-based attention training (Bonavita et al. 2015), and the use of context and imagery (story memory technique) (N. D. Chiaravalloti et al. 2013). Prior to recommending comprehensive neuropsychological testing in PwMS, how testing may inform management and what resources are available after testing to support PwMS requires consideration. One of the earliest and largest randomized controlled trials investigating CI in PwMS found that comprehensive neuropsychological testing was associated with worsening quality of life and mood symptoms (Lincoln 2002). There is a need for more research to guide evidence-based recommendations appropriate for the neuropsychological profiles of PwMS with respect to both CI assessment and treatment.
There are patient and family resources aimed to help with managing CI in MS (Multiple Sclerosis Society of Canada, n.d.; LaRocca and King 2016).1,2 These resources have similarities to patient resources developed for acquired brain injury, stroke, and dementia, in that compensatory strategies, mental health and health behaviour suggestions are frequently included (Heart and Stroke Canada, n.d.; Ontario Neurotrauma Foundation 2020; Saskatchewan Health ABI, n.d.; Alzheimer Society, n.d.). A patient resource from the Multiple Sclerosis International Federation conveniently combines recommendations for cognitive and mood symptoms into one resource for PwMS (MS International Federation, n.d.).3 Patient resources importantly help to dispel the myth that cognition is spared in MS, yet additional measures are needed to help address the challenge of CI in MS.
A lack of rigour in controlling for confounders or moderators of cognitive function, including baseline CI, mood, fatigue, and fatigability in PwMS, is a limitation of the research. The literature search date for the first edition of this module has an end date of July of 2020. Despite these limitations, there exists a large variety of non-pharmacological interventions reporting improvement on objective cognitive outcomes in PwMS. For a review of the pharmacological interventions for cognition in MS and further information on the prevalence, measurement, and impact of CI in MS, please visit the module on Cognitive Pharmacological. The present module provides a review of the non-pharmacological interventions trialed in MS.
2.0 Cognitive Outcome Measures and Defining Cognitive Impairment
Please refer to the module on Cognitive Impairment: Pharmacological Interventions for an introductory text summary on outcome measures.
Table 1. Cognitive Outcome Measures Utilized in the Reviewed Literature for non-pharmacological interventionsCognitive domain | Outcome measure |
Attention
|
Attention Network Test (ANT) Brickenkamp d2 Test (Bd2T) Brief Test of Attention (BTA) Cogstate Brief Battery (CBB) Conner's Continuous Performance Test (CCPT) Continuous Performance Test (CPT) Integrated Auditory Visual-2 (IVA-2) Leiter-3: attention Stroop Attention Scale (SAS) Stroop Test/Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT) Test of Attentional Performance (TAP) Urban Daily Cog |
Executive function Cognitive interference & mental flexibility Cognitive reasoning |
Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult (BRIEF-A) Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS)2 Dysexecutive Syndrome Questionnaire (DEQ): clinician rated Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe) Hayling and Brixton Test (HBT) Self-Regulation Skill (SRSI) Tower of London-II (TOL-II/TOW-II) Flanker Task (FT) Stroop Test/Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT) Trail Making Test - B (TMT -B) Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM) Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices (RCPM) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) |
Information processing speed Auditory processing speed Visual processing speed |
Paced Visual Serial Addition Test (PVSAT) Paced Visual Serial Addition Test-III (PVSAT-III) Trail Making Test - A (TMT -A) Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)2,3 Digit Symbol Substitution (DSST) Faces Symbol Test (FST) Salthouse Perceptual Comparison Test (PCT) Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)1,2,3 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) Digit Symbol |
Visuospatial skills Spatial processing Visual perception |
WAIS-R: Block design subtest Judgment of Line Orientation (JLO) |
Memory Spatial memory Visuospatial memory Visual memory Verbal learning & memory Verbal memory Working memory Autobiography memory |
10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36;10/36-SPART; SPART)3 7/24 Spatial Recall Test Location Learning Task (LLT) Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test – Third Edition (RBMT-3) The Novel Task Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)1,2 Brief Visuospatial Memory Test (BVMT) Door and People Test (DPT) Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF or CFT) Contextual Memory Text (CMT) Doors and People Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT)1,2 California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II)1,2 Greek Verbal Learning Test (GVLT) Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT) Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R) Regensburger Verbal Fluency Test (RVFT) Selective Reminding Test (SRT)3 Verbal Learning and Memory Test (VLMT) Word List Generation Test (WLGT)3 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS) Wechsler Memory Scale-III (WMS-III) Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (RBMT) – Story Memory 2-back Cogstate Brief Battery (CBB) Corsi block-tapping Test (CT/CORSI) Memory Assessment Scale (MAS) Memory for Intentions Test (MIST) National Adult Reading Test (NART) N-back Selective Reminding Prospective Memory paradigm (SRPM) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) digit span Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) digit span Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) letter-number sequencing Autobiographical Interview (AI) Cue-word Modified Crovtiz Test (MCT) Galton-Crovitz Cue-word Test-Modified (GCCW-M) |
Verbal language skills Word retrieval Verbal fluency |
Boston Naming Test (BNT) Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) Animal Fluency (AF) Bilan Informatisé d'Aphasie (BIA) Calibrated Ideational Fluency Assessment (CIFA) Gottschalk and Gleser Measure Isaacs Set Test (IST) Phonemic Fluency Test (PFT) |
General Cognition |
Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination (ACE) Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) Mindstream Computerized Cognitive Test (MCCT) Neuropsychological assessment battery (NAB) Shipley Institute of Living Scales (SILS) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) Woodcock Johnson Test-Revised (WJ-R) Abbreviated Mental Test (AMT) Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) Global Intelligence Efficiency Test (GIET) Multiple Sclerosis Inventory for Cognition (MUSIC) Brief International Cognitive Assessment for Multiple Sclerosis (BiCAMS) Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in Multiple Sclerosis (MACFIMS) Rao's Brief Repeatable (BRB) |
This section includes a collection of studies where authors include mixed strategies or compare broadly different cognitive rehabilitation approaches. Studies targeting or aiming to test a specific cognitive rehabilitation strategy (i.e., self-generation, spaced learning, music mnemonics, etc…) are described separately in each applicable section of this module.
Table 2. Studies Examining Cognitive Rehabilitation for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis (non-computer-based approaches)
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Brissart et al. 2020 Memory improvement in multiple sclerosis after an extensive cognitive rehabilitation program in groups with a multicenter double-blind randomized trial FranceRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=128, NFinal=94 |
Population: Intervention group (n=52): Mean age=47.2yr; Sex: males=14, females=38; Disease course: RRMS=9; Mean EDSS=3.5; Mean disease duration=11.3yr.
Control group (n=49): Mean age=44.9yr; Sex: males=9, females=40; Disease course: RRMS=13; Mean EDSS=3.4; Mean disease duration=12.4yr
Intervention: Following randomization, both groups completed 13, 2-hr sessions over 6mos. The intervention group completed an extended cognitive rehabilitation program, ProCog-SEP, which uses facilitation and reorganization strategies. Facilitation aims to improve cognitive abilities through exercises that target episodic memory, working memory, executive function, and language. Reorganization aims to identify cognitive treatments the patient has not used or only infrequently used and then begin using those treatments. The control group completed non-cognitive exercises with discussion. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and at the end of the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Selective Reminding Test (SRT)1; 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART); Test of Attentional Performance (TAP)2; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III): Digit Symbol and Digit Span2; Bilan Informatisé d'Aphasie (BIA): Letter M and Animals.2 |
|
Martin et al. 2014 Group-based memory rehabilitation for people with multiple sclerosis: subgroup analysis of the ReMiND trial UKRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=39, NFinal=39 |
Population: Compensation group (n=12): Mean age=48.3yr; Gender: males=3, females=9; Disease course: unspecified; Disease severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=131.5mo.
Restitution group (n=17): Mean age=45.2yr; Gender: males=4, females=13; Disease course: unspecified; Disease severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=100.8mo.
Control group (n=17): Mean age=47.7yr; Gender: males=3, females=7; Disease course: unspecified; Disease severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=95.7mo.
Intervention: MS participants were randomized to receive group-based memory rehabilitation, either compensation-based or restitution-based, or to the self-help control condition. The compensation group were taught to use external memory aids while the restitution group completed exercises related to attention, encoding, and retrieval. The self-help control group learned relaxation techniques and coping mechanisms to deal with their condition. Each rehabilitation programme consisted of 10 weekly sessions of 1.5hrs each for 10wks. Assessments were performed at baseline and at 5- and 7-mo follow-up. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Everyday Memory Questionnaire (EMQ)1; Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (RBMT)2; Internal and External Memory Aids questionnaires.2 |
|
Lamargue et al. 2020 Effect of cognitive rehabilitation on neuropsychological and semiecological testing and on daily cognitive functioning in multiple sclerosis: The REACTIV randomized controlled study FranceRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=35, NFinal=35 |
Population: Specific Cognitive Rehabilitation group (n=18): Mean age=43.8yr; Sex: males=6, females=12; Disease course: RRMS=14, PPMS=1, SPMS=3; Median EDSS=3; Mean disease duration=6.7yr.
Nonspecific intervention group (n=17): Mean age=38.3yr; Sex: males=3, females=14; Disease course: RRMS=15, PPMS=1, SPMS=1; Median EDSS=2; Mean disease duration=6.5yr.
Healthy subjects' group (n=21): Mean age=39.7yr; Sex: males=4, females=17.
Intervention: Following randomization, both groups completed 50 individual sessions, 3x/wk for 17wks. The specific cognitive rehabilitation group completed the REACTIV program which focused on fundamental cognitive processes including attention and reaction time. Visual and auditory modalities were used throughout the intervention. The program was progressive and adjusted its level of difficulty based on performance. The nonspecific intervention group received information on MS, coaching on physical activity, and global cognitive stimulation on semantic memory, autobiographical memory, and verbal and visual episodic memory. The healthy subjects group received no intervention. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, 4mos post-treatment, and at 8-mo f/u. MRI measures were collected at baseline. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Test of Attentional Performance (TAP); California Verbal-Learning Test (CVLT); Stroop Test (SCWT); Trail Making Test (TMT); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF); Baddley's Dual Task; Daily Cognitive Activities Questionnaire (DCAQ); Urban Daily Cog.3 |
|
Mani et al. 2018 Efficacy of group cognitive rehabilitation therapy in Multiple Sclerosis IranRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=34, NFinal=30 |
Population: Intervention group (n=17): Mean age=35.29yr; Sex: males=0, females=17; Disease course: RRMS, Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified.
Control group (n=17): Mean age=35.82yr; Sex: males=0, females=17; Disease course: RRMS, Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified.
Intervention: Following randomization, both groups received 2, 2-hr sessions/wk for 4 wks. The intervention group received the cognitive rehabilitation intervention in a group setting. The first session focused on education of the impact of MS on cognitive performance, taught the information processing model, and assigned homework with the direction to write about the impact of MS on their daily cognitive function. The second session focused on the effect of MS on mood and cognition with a homework assignment on documenting daily activities for the week. The third session focused on compensatory attention rehabilitation. Sessions four to seven focused on memory rehabilitation. The eighth session focused on executive function. The control group received a psycho-education intervention as a sham intervention in comparison to the cognition-targeted program of the intervention group. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, at the end of the intervention and at 3-mo f/u. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination (ACE); Continuous Performance Test (CPT); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST); Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult (BRIEF-A); Memory Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ); Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R).3 |
|
Mousavi et al. 2018 Memory rehabilitation for the working memory of patients with multiple sclerosis (MS) IranRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=60, NFinal=60 |
Population: Intervention group (n=20): Mean age=40.55yr; Disease course: not specified; EDSS=<4; Mean disease duration=6.2yr.
Placebo (relaxation) group (n=20): Mean age=41.25yr; Disease course: not specified; EDSS=<4; Mean disease duration=7.55yr.
Control group (n=20): Mean age=40.65yr; Disease course: not specified; EDSS=<4; Mean disease duration=6.8yr.
Intervention: Following randomization, the intervention group completed weekly, 1-hr sessions for 8wks. Each session consisted of introduction of the memory aids, compensatory strategies, mental review methods, error-free learning, focused attention and concentration, and coping mechanisms for memory problems. A homework assignment was assigned at the end of each session. The placebo group completed a relaxation technique at the same frequency. The control group had no intervention. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, after the intervention and 5wks post-intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Wechsler Memory Scale-III (WMS-III).3 |
|
Lincoln et al. 2020 Cognitive rehabilitation for attention and memory in people with multiple sclerosis: a randomized controlled trial (CRAMMS) UKRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=449, NFinal=387 |
Population: Intervention group (n=245): Mean age=49.9yr; Sex: males=67, females=178; Disease course: RRMS=159, PPMS=22, SPMS=64; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=12.1yr.
Control group (n=204): Mean age=48.9yr; Sex: males=56, females=148; Disease course: RRMS=132, PPMS=24, SPMS=48; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=11.1yr.
Intervention: The intervention group received cognitive rehabilitation in a group setting weekly for 10 sessions. The intervention included attention and memory restitution and encoding and retrieval strategies. Homework assignments were assigned to encourage individualization of care and incorporation of cognitive strategies to daily life. The control group received usual care protocol. This included general advice on MS management from a nurse and cognitive deficits management from an occupational therapist. They were also notified of services available through MS charities. Participants nominated a relative or friend to complete the Everyday Memory Questionnaire-relative version for them. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, 6-mo f/u and 12-mo f/u. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N)2 (Selective Reminding Test (SRT); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3 (PASAT-3); Symbol Digit Modalities (SDMT)); Word Fluency; Doors and People Test2; Trail Making Test (TMT)2; Everyday Memory Questionnaire.2 |
|
Kahraman et al. 2020 Physical, cognitive, and psychosocial effects of telerehabilitation-based motor imagery training in people with multiple sclerosis: A randomized controlled pilot trial TurkeyRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=50, NFinal=33 |
Population: Intervention group (n=20): Median age=34.5yr; Sex: males=4, females=16; Disease course: unspecified; Median EDSS=1; Mean disease duration=4yr.
Control group (n=15): Median age=36yr; Sex: males=1, females=14; Disease course: unspecified; Median EDSS=2; Mean disease duration=4yr.
Healthy control group (n=20): Median age=31yr; Sex: males=6, females=14.
Intervention: Following randomization, the intervention group received 2 individualized, physiotherapist-run sessions/wk for 8wks of the telerehabilitation-based motor imaging training (Tele-MIT) intervention. During the training, they used the Physical Environment, Task, Timing, Learning, Emotion, Perspective model which encourages motor imagery to maximize function equivalence. The sessions include relaxation exercises, multisensory environment information, and MIT training. Sessions were progressed as participants improved. A waitlist control group was used in this study. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. The healthy controls only completed the outcome measures once. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)2; Selective Reminding (SRT)2; 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART).2 |
|
Aguirre et al. 2019 Repeated Working Memory Training Improves Task Performance and Neural Efficiency in Multiple Sclerosis Patients and Healthy Controls SpainRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=58, NFinal=27 |
Population: MS untrained group (n=14): Mean age=36.14yr; Sex: males=3, females=11; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=1.8; Mean disease duration=7.54yr.
MS trained group (n=15): Mean age=35.8yr; Sex: males=7, females=8; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=1.67; Mean disease duration=8.33yr.
Healthy control untrained group (n=xx): Mean age=34.13yr; Sex: males=9, females=6.
Healthy control trained group (n=xx): Mean age=31.21yr; Sex: males=6, females=8.
Intervention: Participants were randomized into four groups. The trained groups received the N-back training protocol and completed 4, 60-min sessions over the course of 1wk. The first phase included working memory training exercises over eight blocks that varied in load (1-back, 2-back, 3-back). Participants were asked to respond with only their right hand, using the thumb to respond to targets and the forefinger to nontargets. At the end of the block participants were given information on their correct responses and reaction times. During the test phase, the participants completed 8 blocks of the 2-back and 3-back tasks but were not provided feedback. The non-trained group did nothing during the intervention. fMRI data was collected at baseline, post-intervention, and 42d later. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: N-back task.3 |
|
Shahpouri et al. 2019 Evaluation of cognitive rehabilitation on the cognitive performance in multiple sclerosis: A randomized controlled trial. IranRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=66, NFinal=56 |
Population: Intervention group (n=28): Mean age=32.21yr; Sex: males=8, females=20; Disease course: RRMS=19, PPMS=3, SPMS=6; Mean EDSS=2.28; Mean disease duration=7.46yr.
Control group (n=28): Mean age=30.46yr; Sex: males=9, females=19; Disease course: RRMS=20, PPMS=3, SPMS=5; Mean EDSS=1.87; Mean disease duration=7.07yr.
Intervention: Following randomization, the intervention group completed 10, 2-hr sessions of cognitive rehabilitation over 8wks. The program targeted attention, concentration, visual memory, auditory memory, and autobiographical memory. A mnemonic approach that included visual imagery, theological organization, and relational strategies was used during the session. Autobiographical memory training and effects on daily memory disturbances were also discussed. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 3mos following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Abbreviated Mental Test (AMT); Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ); Everyday Memory Questionnaire (EMQ); Digit Span.3 |
|
Rilo et al. 2018 Integrative group-based cognitive rehabilitation efficacy in multiple sclerosis: a randomized clinical trial SpainRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=44, NFinal=42 |
Population: Intervention group (n=21): Mean age=43.9yr; Sex: males=8, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=15, PPMS=1, SPMS=5; Mean EDSS=3.52; Mean disease duration=9.95yr.
Control group (n=21): Mean age=43.67yr; Sex: males=7, females=14; Disease course: RRMS=17, SPMS=4; Mean EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration=10.67yr.
Intervention: Following randomization, the intervention group completed 3, 1-hr sessions/wk over 3mos and received homework to be completed 3x/wk. The intervention group completed the group-based REHACOP protocol. The REHACOP protocol trains attention, processing speed, working memory, language, executive function, and social cognition. REHACOP has been used to treat CI in Parkinson's disease and schizophrenia. The study used a waitlist control group. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Test of Attention (BTA); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A); Salthouse Perceptual Comparison Test (PCT); Wecshler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III): Digit Span Backwards; Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R); Calibrated Ideational Fluency Assessment (CIFA); Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT).3 |
|
Carr et al. 2014 Group memory rehabilitation for people with multiple sclerosis: a feasibility randomized controlled trial UKRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=48, NFinal=31 |
Population: Treatment group (n=24): Mean age=55.8yr; Gender: males=7, females=17; Disease course: RRMS=7, PPMS=6, SPMS=4, benign=2, unknown=5; Mean EDSS: unspecified; Mean disease duration=16.3yr.
Control group (n=24): Mean age=52.9yr; Gender: males=8, females=16; Disease course: RRMS=9, PPMS=10, SPMS=4, unknown=1; Mean EDSS: Unspecified; Mean disease duration=12.3yr.
Intervention: MS participants with reported memory difficulties received a group memory rehabilitation programme of ten 1.5-hr sessions for 10wks. The programme focused on attention training, internal memory strategies, and external memory aids with a combination of compensatory techniques and restitution. The control group received no treatment. Assessments were administered 4 and 8mos after randomization. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Everyday Memory Questionnaire (EMQ): self report, carer report.1 |
|
Jønsson et al. 1993 Effects of neuropsychological treatment in patients with multiple sclerosis DenmarkRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=40, NFinal=40 |
Population: Total population: Disease course: RRMS=6, SPMS=25, PPMS=9.
Group 1 - Specific Cognitive Training and Psychotherapy (n=20): Mean age=46.1yr; Gender: males=11, females=9; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=5.6; Mean disease duration=15.0yr.
Group 2 - Non-specific Mental Stimulation 2 (n=20): Mean age=43.0yr; Gender: males=10, females=10; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=5.6; Mean disease duration=15.1yr
Intervention: MS patients were randomized to receive either specific cognitive training and psychotherapy or to non-specific mental stimulation. The specific cognitive training and psychotherapy group received traditional cognitive therapy aimed at restoring concentration, memory, and spatial skills and learning compensatory skills. They also received psychotherapy to improve their coping skills. The non-specific mental stimulation group played games and watched/read and discussed films, literature, and newspaper articles. Both groups were treated for 1 to 1.5hrs 3x/wk for an average duration of 46d. Assessments were performed at baseline, immediately after, and 6mos after treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS): Similarities, Picture Arrangement, Digit Span, and Block Design; Trail Making Test-A and B (TMT); Symbol Digit Modalities (SDMT); STREET incomplete pictures; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Verbal Fluency (Animals and Words); 50 words and 50 Faces; Visual Gestalts Test.3 |
|
Hanssen et al. 2016 Cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis: A randomized controlled trial NorwayRCT PEDro=4 NInitial=120, NFinal=101 |
Population: Intervention group (n=60): Mean age=53.9yr; Gender: males=20, females=40; Disease course: RRMS=27, PPMS=18, SPMS=15; Mean EDSS=4.4; Mean disease duration=10.6yr.
Control group (n=60): Mean age=52.5yr; Gender: males=12, females=48; Disease course: RRMS=32, PPMS=10, SPMS=18; Mean EDSS=4.2; Mean disease duration=12.0yr.
Intervention: MS patients were randomized to receive a cognitive rehabilitation intervention group or a control group. Both groups received ordinary MS inpatient rehabilitation for 4wks, which consisted of education and physical activities. The intervention group also participated in group and individual rehabilitation sessions for 4mos for a total of about 15hrs of total therapy, which focused on compensatory techniques aimed at executive functioning. The intervention group also received psychotherapy. Assessments were performed at baseline, and at 4 and 7mos from the start of the study. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult (BRIEF-A): Global executive composite score (GEC), Intelligence quotient Global executive composite score (GEC IQ), Metacognitive index (MI); General executive composite (GEC BRIEF-A).1 |
|
Zuber et al. 2020 Efficacy of inpatient personalized multidisciplinary rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis: behavioural and functional imaging results SwitzerlandPCT PEDro=8 NInitial=48, NFinal=48 |
Population: MS Participants (n=24): Mean age=47.7yr; Sex: males=8, females=16; Disease course: Relapse onset MS; Median EDSS=5; Mean disease duration=15.2yr.
Non-MS Control group (n=24): Mean age=45yr; Sex: males=8, females=16.
Intervention: The participants with MS took part in a personalized 4-wk inpatient multidisciplinary rehabilitation program. They received an average 16.6d of rehabilitation lasting an average of 46.1hrs. The interdisciplinary approach was based on the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health and included the development of personalized goals. Both individual and group therapy settings were utilized in the study. The non-MS control group received no intervention. Outcome and MRI measures for both groups were collected at baseline and following the intervention, and the intervention group had one more session at 4-wk f/u. During the fMRI session, participants had to complete a motor sequence learning task. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: : Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT); oral version of Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV): Digit Span; Corsi Block-Tapping Test (CORSI). 3 |
|
Brissart et al. 2013 Cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis FrancePCT PEDro=8 NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: Treatment Group (n=10): Mean age=42.5yr; Gender unspecified; Disease course: RRMS=10; Mean EDSS=2.9; Mean disease duration=5yr.
Control Group (n=10): Mean age=41.3yr; Gender unspecified; Disease course: RRMS=10; Mean EDSS=2.85; Mean disease duration=7.2yr.
Intervention: The treatment group underwent the ProCogSEP cognitive rehabilitation program consisting of 2 hrs/session for 13 sessions over 6mos. ProCogSEP has 13 modules, which focus on semantic memory, visual memory, verbal memory, working memory, and executive function. The control group followed 13, 2-hr sessions of neutral discussion and non-cognitive exercises. All patients received assessments at baseline and 3mos after treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Selective Reminding Test (SRT): free mean recall, learning percent, delayed free recall; 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART): free recall, delayed Recall; Test of Attentional Performance (TAP): correct responses, working memory omissions, flexibility errors, incompatibility correct responses; Verbal Fluencies and Semantic Fluencies (Fluencies “M” and Fluencies animals); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III): Digit Span; Boston Naming Test (BNT).3 |
|
Fink et al. 2010 Efficacy of an executive function intervention programme in MS: a placebo-controlled and pseudo-randomized trial GermanyPCT PEDro=7 NInitial=50, NFinal=20 |
Population: Mean age=44.8yr; Gender: males=9, females=41; Disease course: RRMS=50; Severity: Unspecified; Mean disease duration=92.4mo.
Cognitive intervention group (CIG, n=11).
Placebo group (PG, n=14).
Untreated group (UG, n=15).
Intervention: RRMS patients were divided into either a cognitive intervention group (CIG), a placebo group (PG), or an untreated group (UG). Subjects in the CIG and PG completed 6-wk interventions. Patients in the CIG completed 30-min sessions, 4x/wk, on textbook exercises for executive functioning. They also met with a psychologist for 1.5hrs weekly for feedback. Subjects in the PG completed five 40-min sessions/wk of reaction capacity testing using RehaCom software. The UG received no training. Assessments were performed at baseline (t1), after intervention (t2), and 1yr after enrollment (t3). Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Preference Shifting (PS): trials to criterion (TTC), reaction time (RT); Response Shifting (RS): trials to criterion (TTC), reaction time (RT); 2-back: commissions, omissions; California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT): learning; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale: short form (WAIS).3 |
|
Brenk, Laun, and Haase 2008 Short-term cognitive training improves mental efficiency and mood in patients with multiple sclerosis GermanyPre-Post NInitial=41, NFinal=41 |
Population: MS Participants (n=27): Mean age=43.5yr; Gender: males=12, females=15; Disease course: RRMS=27; Severity: Unspecified; Disease duration=3-10yr.
Healthy control group (n=14): Mean age=39.6yr; Gender: males=7, females=7.
Intervention: Participants received home-based non-specific cognitive training for 6wks. The training consisted of 90 small 5-min tasks/wk from Brain Gymnastics (Gripsgymnastik) books, which include crosswords, comparisons, recall of images, and word definitions. These exercises may be completed in approximately 5min. Participants were free to complete the 90 5-min tasks in a single day or over the course of each week. Assessments were performed at baseline (T1) and after treatment (T2) at 6wks. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS): Digit Span; Verbal Learning and Memory Test (VLMT); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (CFT): delayed; Test of Attentional Performance (TAP): tonic and phasic alertness, go/no-go, shared attention; Regensburger Test of Word Fluency (RWT).3 |
|
Gentry 2008 PDAs as cognitive aids for people with multiple sclerosis USAPre-Post NInitial=21, NFinal=20 |
Population: Median age=50yr; Gender: males=4, females=16; Disease course: RRMS=13, PPMS=3, SPMS=3, other=1; Severity: unspecified; Median disease duration=14yr. Intervention: Participants were trained to use Personal Digital Assistants (PDAs) to investigate functional performance which was assessed at the start of the 8-wk pre-treatment period, at the beginning and end of the training, and at 8wks after the conclusion of the training. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test-Extended (RBMT-E); Canadian Occupational Performance Measure (COPM); Craig Handicap Assessment and Rating Technique-Revised (CHART-R).3 |
|
Kardiasmenos et al. 2008 Prospective memory and the efficacy of a memory strategy in multiple sclerosis USAPre-Post NInitial=24, NFinal=24 |
Population: MS participants (n=24): Mean age=44.4yr; Gender: males=13, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=18, SPMS=6; Median EDSS=3.75; Mean disease duration=7.9yr.
Healthy control (n=24): Mean age=42.8yr; Gender: males=12, females=12. Intervention: MS participants received training in implementation intentions, which is a mnemonic strategy. Participants played a board game (“Virtual Week”) that mimics everyday life and requires the use of prospective memory by requiring players to complete future tasks at specific times. Participants were given two types of instructions. When given the rote-rehearsal instructions, participants were told a task to complete, asked to repeat it aloud for 10s, and then told to stop. When given implementation-intentions instructions, participants were told that whenever a task was assigned, they should close their eyes, visualize completing the task, and then state aloud their future intentions. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Mean Correct Proportion of Memory Tasks.3 |
|
Rodgers et al. 1996 Cognitive therapy for multiple sclerosis: a preliminary study USPCT NInitial=27, NFinal=22 |
Population: Therapy group (n=12): Mean age=49.9yr; Gender: males=5, females=7. No further information provided.
Control group (n=10): Mean age=37.8yr; Gender: males=6, females=4. No further information provided. Intervention: Participants in the therapy group received a multimodal educational program consisting of psychotherapy, expressive therapy (art and music), and mind-body approaches using training in self-regulation, visualization techniques, guided imagery, meditation, relaxation, and mental and physical exercises. Participants received therapy across 24 3-hr sessions, occurring once per week. Homework was assigned for 1-1.5hr/d. Participants in the control group received no therapy. Assessments were performed at baseline, 12wks, and 24wks. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Word List Learning and Memory; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Shipley Institute of Living Scales (SILS): vocabulary and abstraction.1 |
|
Summary
Table 3. Summary Table of Studies Examining Cognitive Rehabilitation (non-computer-based approaches)
Improve | No statistical sig. difference | |
General Cognition |
|
|
Attention |
|
|
Executive Function |
|
|
Information Processing |
|
|
Memory |
|
|
Verbal Fluency |
|
|
w | RCT with within-group comparison only |
Bold | PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | PEDro < 6 |
Italic | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Six high-quality RCTs (Brissart et al. 2020; Martin et al. 2014; Lamargue et al. 2020; Mani et al. 2018; Mousavi et al. 2018; Lincoln et al. 2020) investigated various cognitive rehabilitation approaches. Four of these studies included at least some compensatory memory approaches delivered in a group setting (Martin et al. 2014; Mani et al. 2018; Mousavi et al. 2018; Lincoln et al. 2020). Significant improvement on objective memory testing occurred in comparison to the control groups for two RCTs delivering group-based interventions (Mani et al. 2018; Mousavi et al. 2018) and two RCTs delivering individual programs (Brissart et al. 2020; Lamargue et al. 2020). Lower quality studies report mixed results; however, a greater proportion of the lower quality studies also report positive findings for objective memory outcomes. There remain unanswered questions about the most appropriate patient selection for which approaches, the duration and intensity of treatment, and the longer-term benefit. All six of the higher quality RCTs included PwMS with minimum to moderate cognitive impairment in one or more cognitive domain. In one study, the positive results on objective memory testing occurred after only 8 hours of a group-based intervention over four weeks (Mousavi et al. 2018). The focus of the intervention in this study was on coping and compensatory strategies (Mousavi et al. 2018).
From a service delivery perspective, lower cost group-based interventions are feasible and acceptable for many PwMS. Patient self-reported cognitive functioning may improve significantly with group-based memory restitution and encoding strategy training, even when improvement on the objective cognitive outcomes may not reach statistical significance (Lincoln et al. 2020). A multimodal approach involving physiotherapy, psychotherapy, and an imagery intervention compared to no intervention improved objective memory outcomes when this type of multi-modal intervention was delivered in person (Rodgers et al. 1996) or by tele-rehabilitation (Kahraman et al. 2020).
When interpreting the results of the MS cognitive rehabilitation research, heterogeneity across studies, effect sizes within groups, and other design issues warrant consideration. In the Lincoln et al. (2020) high-quality RCT, the intervention group (receiving focused memory training) performed significantly better than the control group on only two of the eight objective cognitive subtests. However, the control group received “usual care”, including access to MS nursing support and occupational therapy. Objective cognitive test scores still improved over the course of the study in both the intervention and the usual care control groups. Control groups where participants may access health professionals with expertise in cognitive rehabilitation may diminish the power to detect between-group differences on cognitive outcomes.
Martin et al. (2014) was also a high-quality RCT with negative results for between-group differences on objective cognitive outcomes. Martin et al. (2014) report the MS participant results from the ReMIND trial (das Nair and Lincoln 2012), which is a large trial including people with stroke, acquired brain injury, and MS. The primary objective of the ReMIND trial was to compare compensatory versus restitution approaches in cognitive rehabilitation. The trial included three arms: one group received compensatory approaches; one group received restitution exercises related to attention, encoding, and retrieval; and the control group learned relaxation techniques and coping mechanisms. Importantly, all three groups improved on objective memory outcomes. The restitution group also demonstrated increased self-reported use of internal memory aids, and more symptoms of emotional distress at the five- and seven-month follow-up. The possible positive confounding effects of self-help and relaxation in the control group for improving cognitive performance and the increased emotional distress in the restitution group are of clinical relevance. The ReMIND trial does not provide evidence that one cognitive rehabilitation approach over another is more effective at improving objective cognitive outcomes. However, the results do suggest that stressors and response to stress may be important when choosing strategies to optimize self-reported functioning and objective memory.
The results are more conflicting for objective cognitive outcomes in cognitive domains beyond memory. This may be because cognitive rehabilitation may not be as effective for improving cognition in other cognitive domains in PwMS, active control groups diminish the power to detect between-group differences, and studies may differently emphasize cognitive rehabilitation techniques known to influence learning. For example, spaced practice is more effective at improving learning than mass practice (Sumowski, Chiaravalloti, and DeLuca 2010; Sumowski et al. 2013; Yael Goverover et al. 2009) (see section 3.8 of this module).
Task-specific training or cognitive domain-specific training may be more likely to improve testing in the trained domain. Lamargue et al. (2020) targeted attention training using the REACTIV program for the intervention group while the control group received non-specific cognitive and physical activity coaching. Attention improved in the REACTIV intervention group compared to the control group, yet executive function did not (Lamargue et al. 2020). Fink et al. (2010) compared a group receiving targeted executive function training to a control group receiving only reaction time training. Greater improvement in executive function occurred in the group receiving targeted executive function training. In future research and clinical practice, individualizing and aligning the cognitive rehabilitation training with the goals of treatment is more likely to lead to best outcomes.
The focus of this module is on objective cognitive outcomes as the gold standard for assessing cognition. The mechanisms resulting in improved objective cognitive function are also of interest. Zuber et. al. (2020) completed a pre-post study examining the effects of an individualized cognitive and physical rehabilitation program among PwMS on fMRI and a battery of cognitive outcomes. Improvements on the cognitive outcomes occurred after 4 weeks of rehabilitation for some of the objective cognitive outcomes (i.e., the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test and Digit Span), but not all (i.e., Symbol Digit Modalities Test and Corsi Block-Tapping Test). Post rehabilitation, PwMS demonstrated improved accuracy on a cognitive motor learning sequence task whereby at the same time, on fMRI a decrease in brain activity was observed in the left cerebellum and right frontal lobe. Self-reported fatigue symptoms and walking speed also improved over the four weeks of inpatient rehabilitation. Authors suggest that inpatient rehabilitation training may improve the efficiency by which PwMS complete cognitive motor tasks. Similar improved efficiencies on fMRI are described with purely cognitive tasks after cognitive rehabilitation training in PwMS (Olga Boukrina et al. 2020; A. Ernst et al. 2018) (see also section 3.13, Mental Visual Imagery).
No other studies with strictly inpatient rehabilitation settings met the inclusion criteria for this module, with the exception the above-mentioned Zuber et al. (2020) study. However, information about the effectiveness of inpatient rehabilitation services on cognition in PwMS may be helpful for setting realistic expectations and developing best practices. An inpatient case series by Andrews and Middleton (2018) from Australia included over 1400 PwMS. They reported on the Functional Independence Measure (FIM) cognitive sub-score changes from admission to discharge (Andrews and Middleton 2018). Mean FIM cognitive sub-scores for PwMS improved by 37.5%, which was comparable to the improvement observed for acute stroke patients. To calculate the FIM cognitive sub-score, the clinician evaluates the patient's level of assistance required for comprehension, expression, social interaction, problem solving, and memory (Zeltzer, Korner-Bitensky, and Sitcoff, n.d.). The FIM is not a direct objective measure of cognition, and therefore the case series by Andrews and Middleton (2018) did not meet the inclusion criteria for the MSBEST module. The FIM provides an estimate of the demands on the caregiver. Inpatient rehabilitation services may vary considerably for PwMS, especially since MS best practice inpatient rehabilitation guidelines are not established. Appropriate selection for inpatient services may be critical, as is the case for stroke rehabilitation (Canadian Stroke Best Practices 2019). However, it is encouraging that PwMS improved on the FIM cognitive sub-scores in this large case series study (Andrews and Middleton 2018).
Testing in different paradigms may help to understand the real-world implications of cognitive rehabilitation. Lamargue et al. (2020) included a virtual reality cognitive testing paradigm using the Urban Daily Cog. The virtual Urban Daily Cog task in this study involved viewing a screen with roads and responding to traffic lights. The intervention group (receiving specific attention and reaction time training) improved in their accuracy on this virtual task in comparison to an active control group—despite the fact that the two groups performed similarly on the majority of the other objective cognitive outcomes (Lamargue et al. 2020).
Virtual reality training or testing paradigms may not yet be readily accessible for PwMS, yet may show promise for improving cognitive functioning and outcomes (see section 3.4 Virtual Reality). Digital technology is widely accessed by a large portion of the MS population, especially among those with lower levels of disability (Remy et al. 2018). In a study by Gentry et al. (2008), PwMS with memory impairment at baseline were trained to use a Personal Digital Assistant (PDA) to help with everyday tasks. There was no improvement on the objective cognitive outcomes, yet there was significant improvement on measures of patient- and clinician-rated functional performance.
The interventions for cognitive rehabilitation are heterogeneous and the results of objective cognitive outcomes may not correlate with the patient or caregiver experience. Interventions in which both objective measures as well as patient- and caregiver-reported outcomes improve are of particular interest. There is high-level evidence from four high-quality RCTs and from lower quality studies supporting that mixed cognitive rehabilitation approaches incorporating various strategies improve objective memory outcomes in PwMS.
Conclusion
Attention
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves attention (three randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Brenk et al. 2008, Lamargue et al. 2020, Mani et al. 2018, and Rilo et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that the REACTIV program, which targets attention, may improve attention more than non-specific cognitive exercises persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Lamargue et al. 2020).
There is level 2 evidence that the REHACOP protocol may not improve attention more than no treatment (one randomized controlled trial; Rilo et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that memory and attention rehabilitation using education and compensatory strategies may not improve attention more than sham psychoeducation in persons with MS with cognitive impairment (one randomized controlled trial; Mani et al. 2018).
Memory
There is level 1b evidence that the French ProCog-SEP involving facilitation and reorganization training improves memory in persons with MS with cognitive impairment more than non-cognitive training and discussion (one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Brissart et al. 2020 and Brissart et al. 2013).
There is level 2 evidence that n-back training over the course of 1 week may improve working memory in persons with MS with cognitive impairment compared to no treatment (one randomized controlled trial; Aguirre et al. 2019).
There is level 2 evidence that memory improves more with the REHACOP protocol compared to no treatment (one randomized controlled trial; Rilo et al. 2013).
There is level 1b evidence that the REACTIV protocol may improve verbal learning and memory but not other aspects of memory persons with MS with cognitive impairment (one randomized controlled trial; Lamargue et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that compensatory strategies targeting attention and memory may improve memory more than no treatment in persons with MS (Mousavi et al. 2018 and Mousavi et al. 2018b).
There is level 1b evidence that compensatory memory strategies may not improve memory more than restitution in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (one randomized controlled trial; Martin et al. 2014).
There is level 4 evidence that practicing mental imagery with mnemonic memory techniques together may improve prospective memory when playing a board game in minimally cognitive-impaired persons with MS more than in healthy controls (one pre-post study; Kardiasmenos et al. 2008).
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves memory in persons with MS among studies with different rehabilitation interventions, comparator groups, and memory outcomes (twelve randomized controlled trials, four prospective controlled trials, and one pre-post study; Aguirre et al. 2019, Brenk et al. 2008, Brissart et al. 2013, Brissart et al. 2020, Carr et al. 2014, Fink et al. 2010, Goverover et al. 2009, Jonsson et al. 1993, Kahraman et al. 2020, Lamargue et al. 2020, Lincoln et al. 2020, Mani et al. 2018, Martin et al. 2014, Mousavi et al. 2018, Rilo et al. 2018, Rodgers et al., 1996, and Shahpouri et al. 2019).
Executive Function
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves executive function in persons with MS (four randomized controlled trials, one prospective controlled trial, and one pre-post study; Brenk et al. 2008, Fink et al. 2010, Hanssen et al. 2016, Lincoln et al. 2002, Mani et al. 2018, and Rilo et al., 2018).
There is conflicting evidence whether memory and attention cognitive rehabilitation combined with compensatory strategies improves executive function in persons with MS (two randomized controlled trials; Lincoln et al. 2002 and Mani et al. 2018).
There is level 2 evidence that cognitive rehabilitation targeting executive function may not improve executive function more than normal MS rehab and physiotherapy (one randomized controlled trial; Hanssen et al. 2016).
There is level 2 evidence that the REHACOP protocol may improve executive function more than no treatment persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Rilo et al. 2018).
There is level 2 evidence that executive functioning training using executive function textbook exercises may improve executive function in relapsing-remitting MS more than RehaCom reaction time training or no treatment (one prospective controlled trial; Fink et al. 2010).
Information Processing
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves information processing speed persons with MS (five randomized controlled trials, one prospective trial study, and one pre-post study; Brissart et al. 2020, Kahraman et al. 2020, Lamargue et al. 2020, Lincoln et al. 2020, Rilo et al. 2018, Rodgers et al., 1996, and Zuber et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that the French ProCog-SEP program involving facilitation and reorganization training may not improve information processing speed more than non-cognitive exercises and discussion in relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial, Brissart et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that the REACTIV program may not improve information processing more than non-specific cognitive training and physical activity (one randomized controlled trial; Lamargue et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that group cognitive training that focuses on compensatory strategies and restitution for memory and attention may not improve information processing speed compared to usual care (defined as advice from nursing and OT) in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Lincoln et al. 2020).
There is level 2 evidence that the REHACOP protocol may improve information processing more than no treatment persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Rilo et al. 2018).
There is level 2 evidence that Tele-MIT may improve information processing more than no treatment in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Kahraman et al. 2020).
Verbal Language
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves verbal language skills persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Brissart et al. 2013 and Rilo et al. 2013).
Summary
Rehabilitation approaches targeting memory improve memory in persons with MS with minimum to moderate cognitive impairment compared to no treatment.
Restitution approaches may increase self-reported stress levels compared to compensatory approaches.
Non-specific or multi-modal rehabilitation approaches delivered individually, in a group, or remotely may improve memory in persons with MS compared to no treatment.
Rehabilitation approaches targeting executive function improve executive function outcomes in persons with MS with minimum cognitive impairment.
Compensatory approaches targeting memory in persons with MS may not be superior to restitution approaches, self-management coaching, or access to MS occupational therapy and nursing services for improving memory.
Non-specific cognitive rehabilitation approaches may not improve outcomes in other cognitive domains besides memory compared to no treatment.
This section includes research studies evaluating the potential benefit of a computer-based cognitive intervention, either in comparison to no treatment, to another computer-based intervention, or to other non-computer-based cognitive rehabilitation approaches.
The distinction between cognitive computer training and gaming is not always obvious in the literature. Several studies clearly set out to examine the effects of gaming. We include these few gaming studies in a separate extraction table following the computer training studies (See section 3.3, Video Games, table 6).
Table 4. Studies Examining Computer-based Training for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Chiaravalloti et al. 2018 A Pilot Study Examining Speed of Processing Training (SPT) to Improve Processing Speed in Persons With Multiple Sclerosis USARCT PEDro=9 NInitial=21, NFinal=21 |
Population: Intervention group (n=12): Mean age=46.42yr; Sex: males=3, females=9; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=12.7yr.
Control group (n=9): Mean age=52.11yr; Sex: males=3, females=6; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=3.4yr. Intervention: Following randomization, the intervention group completed 10 computerized sessions of Speed of Processing Training (SPT) over a 5-wk period. The intervention involved three levels. Level 1 involved a single discrimination task that was to be completed at a progressively faster speed. Level 2 involved a discrimination task to be completed while locating a peripheral target. Level 3 involved the same tasks as level 2, but the peripheral target was hidden among distractors. Each level continued until the participants got 75% correct. The control group received no treatment. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 1wk following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III): Digit Symbol1; Letter Comparison (LC)2; Pattern Comparison (PC)2; California Learning Verbal Test II (CVLT-II).2 |
|
De Luca et al. 2019 Do people with multiple sclerosis benefit from PC based neurorehabilitation? A pilot study. ItalyRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=40, NFinal=40 |
Population: Intervention group (n=20): Mean age=52.7yr; Sex: males=12, females=8; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=5.1; Mean disease duration=8.3yr.
Control group (n=20): Mean age=57.0yr; Sex: males=11, females=9; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=5.1; Mean disease duration=8.9yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups received 3, 45-min sessions/wk for 8wks. Participants in the computer-based intervention used the ERICA platform. Sessions focused on specific cognitive domains including attention, verbal memory, visuospatial memory, and executive function. Difficulty was adjusted to the patient's performance. The control group completed traditional face-to-face cognitive therapy which was individualized to each patient. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA); Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (Selective Reminding (SRT), 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3 (PASAT-3), Symbol Digit Modalities (SDMT), Word List Generation Test (WLGT)).3 |
|
Stuifbergen et al. 2018 Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation in persons with multiple sclerosis: Results of a multi-site randomized controlled trial with six month follow-up USARCT PEDro=8 NInitial=183, NFinal=163 |
Population: Intervention group (n=93): Mean age=49.8yr; Sex: males=13, females=80; Disease course: RRMS=64, PPMS=3, SPMS=14, PRMS=1; Mean EDSS=5.1; Mean disease duration=13.9yr.
Control group (n=90): Mean age=49.4yr; Sex: males=10, females=80; Disease course: RRMS=61, PPMS=5, SPMS=10, PRMS=1; Mean EDSS=5.3; Mean disease duration=12.1yr. Intervention: Following randomization, the intervention group received the MAPSS-MS program. This included weekly, 2-hr group sessions and 3 daily, 45-min home-based computer training sessions for 6wks. The first four group sessions focused on common cognitive deficits in MS and compensatory strategies. The final four group sessions focused on lifestyle behaviours to support cognitive functioning. The computer protocol used the Lumosity software, which addressed attention, memory, flexibility, and problem solving. The complexity of the tasks adjusted to the participant's performance. The control group underwent usual care and had access to the public computer games “MyBrainGames” on multiplesclerosis.com, which target information processing, attention, and executive function. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, after the intervention (6wks) and at 3- and 6-mo f/u. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in MS (MACFIMS) (Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT), California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)); PROMIS Applied Cognition Abilities Short Form.3 |
|
Charvet et al. 2017 Cognitive function in multiple sclerosis improves with telerehabilitation: Results from a randomized controlled trial USARCT PEDro=8 NInitial=135, NFinal=135 |
Population: Adaptive Cognitive Rehabilitation (ACR) group (n=74): Mean age=48yr; Gender: males=24, females=50; Disease course: RRMS=51, PPMS=3, SPMS=20; Mean EDSS=3.5; Mean disease duration=11.9yr.
Active Control group (n=61): Mean age=52; Gender: males=7, females=54; Disease course: RRMS=39, PPMS=4, SPMS=15; Mean EDSS=3.5; Mean disease duration=13.5yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to receive either Adaptive Cognitive Remediation (ACR) or active control of computer games for 60hr over 12wks. The ACR program was adapted from BrainHQ, an online brain-training service, and included 15 exercises targeting attention, working memory, executive function, and information processing speed. The active control played “Hoyle puzzles and board games,” which included card games such as bridge and board games such as backgammon. Assessments were performed at baseline and after treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Neuropsychological composite score (Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV): letter number sequence, digit span backwards; Selective Reminding Test (SRT); Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R); Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) trails).1 |
|
Charvet et al. 2015 Remotely-delivered cognitive remediation in multiple sclerosis (MS): protocol and results from a pilot study USARCT PEDro=8 NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: Intervention group (n=11): Mean age=38yr; Gender: males=4, females=7; Disease course: RRMS=11; Median EDSS=2; Mean disease duration Unspecified.
Control group (n=9): Mean age=42yr; Gender: males=2, females=7; Disease course: RRMS=9; Median EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration Unspecified. Intervention: RRMS patients were randomized to either a home-based, remotely supervised, active cognitive remediation (ACR) program group, or a control group consisting of ordinary video games. The ACR program used Lumosity, an online platform with brain training games. The control played “Hoyle puzzles and board games,” which included card games such as bridge and board games such as backgammon. Both groups completed 5d of treatment/wk for 12wks in 30-min sessions. Assessments were performed at baseline and after treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: A Cognitive Composite score (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV: letter number sequencing, Selective Reminding Test (SRT), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R), Corsi Block Visual Sequence (CORSI)).2 |
|
Mäntynen et al. 2014 Neuropsychological rehabilitation does not improve cognitive performance but reduces perceived cognitive deficits in patients with multiple sclerosis: a randomised, controlled, multi-centre trial FinlandRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=102, NFinal=98 |
Population: Treatment group (n=58): Mean age=43.5yr; Gender: males=13, females=45; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.2; Mean disease duration=9.2yr.
Control group (n=40): Mean age=44.1yr; Gender: males=9, females=31; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.2; Mean disease duration=10.1yr Intervention: MS patients were randomized to receive either neuropsychological rehabilitation or the control condition. Before intervention, patients set three goals related to attentional problems. These goals were reviewed at the end of rehabilitation. The computer-based neuropsychological rehabilitation consisted of weekly 60-min sessions for 13wks on attention retraining and working memory. In addition, they received compensatory strategies for managing perceived cognitive, mood, and fatigue deficits. Assessments were performed at baseline (T0), 3mos (T1) and 6mos (T2) after initiation. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)1; Perceived Deficits Questionnaire (PDQ)1; Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS)1; Buschkle selective reminding test: long-term storage (BSRT/LTS), consistent long-term retrieval (BSRT/CLTR), Delayed recall (BSRT)2; 10/36 total correct, delayed recall (SPART)2; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 2,3 seconds (PASAT-2, -3)2; Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT)2; Stroop: colour naming time, colour/word interference time2; Trail Making Test A and B (TMT-A, -B)2. |
|
Cerasa et al. 2013 Computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation of attention deficits for multiple sclerosis: a randomized trial with fMRI correlates ItalyRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=26, NFinal=23 |
Population: Experimental Group (n=12): Mean age=31.7yr; Gender: males=3, females=9; Disease course: RRMS=12; Median EDSS=3; Mean disease duration=51.2mos.
Control Group (n=11): Mean age=33.7yr; Gender: males=3, females=8; Disease course: RRMS=11; Median EDSS=2; Mean disease duration=61.6mos. Intervention: RRMS patients were randomized to receive cognitive rehabilitation or placebo intervention. The computer-based cognitive rehabilitation used the RehaCom software, which has modules dedicated to different cognitive domains. This study used the “divided attention,” “attention and concentration,” and “vigilance” modules. Patients met 2x/wk for 1h for 6wks. The control group performed visuomotor computer-based exercises. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and 6wks following the end of the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Selective Reminding Test: long-term storage (SRT-LTS), consistent long-term storage (SRT-CLTR), delayed (SRT-D); Spatial recall test: immediate (SPART-I), delayed (SPART-D); Word List Generation Test (WLGT); Symbol Digit Modality Test (SDMT); Trial Making Test A, B (TMT-A, TMT-B); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3 second (PASAT-3); Stroop Color-Word-Test (SCWT).3 |
|
Stuifbergen et al. 2012 A randomized controlled trial of a cognitive rehabilitation intervention for persons with multiple sclerosis USARCT PEDro=8 NInitial=63, NFinal=61 |
Population: Total population (n=61): Mean age=47.95yr; Gender: males=7, females=54; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=5.2; Mean disease duration=12.2yr. Treatment group (n=34): Gender: males=5, females=29. Control group (n=27): Gender: males=2, females=25.
Intervention: MS patients were randomized to the cognitive rehabilitation treatment group (Memory, Attention, and Problem-Solving Skills for persons with Multiple Sclerosis (MAPSS-MS)) or to the waitlist control group for 8wks of intervention. The intervention consisted of 8 weekly group sessions focused on building efficacy for use of cognitive compensatory strategies and computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation with home-based training. The cognitive rehabilitation exercises were found on www.neuropsychonline.com and focused on attention, executive function, memory, and problem solving. Assessments were performed at baseline, after 8wks of intervention, and 5mos after randomization. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in MS (MACFIMS) (Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT); Judgment of line orientation test (JLO); California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II); Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (DKEFS)).3. |
|
Mattioli et al. 2010 Efficacy and specificity of intensive cognitive rehabilitation of attention and executive functions in multiple sclerosis ItalyRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: Study group (n=10): Median age=44.00yr; Gender: females=10; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration=16.50yr.
Control group (n=10): Median age=42.00yr; Gender: females=10; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=1.5; Mean disease duration=18.50yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to receive a computer-based intensive training program or a control condition. The training program used RehaCom software's “Plan a Day” and “Divided Attention” modules and consisted of 1-hr sessions, 3x/wk for 3mos focusing on attention, information processing, and executive function. The control group did not receive any rehabilitation. Assessments were performed at baseline (T0) and after 3mos of rehabilitation (T1). Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 2, 3 seconds (PASAT-2, -3); Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST) total errors (te), perseverative errors (pe), perseverative responses (pr); Controlled Oral Word Association Test with phonemic (COWA/P) and semantic (COWA/S) cues; Divided attention of test of everyday attention (TEA); Selective Reminding Test: consistent long-term retrieval (SRT-CLTR), delayed recall (SRT-DR); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test: long term retrieval (10/36 SRT LTR), delayed recall (10/36 SRT LTR); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).3 |
|
Solari et al. 2004 Computer-aided retraining of memory and attention in people with multiple sclerosis: A randomized, double-blind controlled trial ItalyRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=82, NFinal=77 |
Population: Intervention group (n=40): Mean age=46.2yr; Gender: males=14, females=26; Disease course: RRMS=17, RPMS=20, CPMS=3; Mean EDSS=3.0; Mean disease duration=15.2yr.
Control group (n=37): Mean age=41.2yr; Gender: males=14, females=23; Disease course: RRMS=22, RPMS=15; Mean EDSS=4.0; Mean disease duration=13.5yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to one of two computer-assisted retraining interventions using RehaCom software. The intervention consisted of memory and attention training, and the control condition consisted of visuo-motor coordination. Both groups received 16 training sessions over 8wks. Assessments were performed before and after 8 and 16wks. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (Selective Reminding Test (SRT): consistent long-term retrieval, delayed recall; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-2 (PASAT-2); Word List Generation Test (WLGT); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART): immediate recall, delayed recall).3 |
|
Messinis et al. 2020 Do Secondary Progressive Multiple Sclerosis patients benefit from Computer-based cognitive neurorehabilitation? A randomized sham controlled trial GreeceRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=36, NFinal=36 |
Population: Intervention group (n=19): Mean age=46.47yr; Sex: males=7, females=12; Disease course: SPMS; Median EDSS=5.5; Mean disease duration=21.15yr.
Control group (n=17): Mean age=45.29yr; Sex: males=5, females=12; Disease course: SPMS; Median EDSS=6.0; Mean disease duration=20.76yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups completed 3, 45-min sessions/wk for 8wks. The intervention was conducted using the home-based RehaCom software, which included 24 individual domain and task-specific sessions. The control group completed a computer-based sham intervention along with their standard clinical therapy. The sham included computer-based brain teasers, newspaper articles, shopping games, etc. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment for Multiple Sclerosis (BiCAMS)1 (Symbol Digits Modalities Test (SDMT), Greek Verbal Learning Test (GVLT), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)). |
|
Campbell et al. 2016 A randomised controlled trial of efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis: A cognitive, behavioural, and MRI study UKRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=38, NFinal=38 |
Population: Treatment group (n=19): Mean age=46.21yr; Gender: males=6, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=14, SPMS=5; Mean EDSS=4.42; Mean disease duration=10.53yr.
Control group (n=19): Mean age=48.53yr; Gender: males=5, females=14; Disease course: RRMS=13, SPMS=6; Mean EDSS=4.45; Mean disease duration=12.68yr. Intervention: Participants with MS and CI were randomized to undergo 45min of computerized cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom software 3x/wk for 6wks, or to the control condition in which patients watched natural history DVDs. The intervention group trained in three RehaCom modules: divided attention, working memory, and topological memory. Assessments were conducted at baseline (T1), after treatment (T2), and at a 12-wk post-treatment f/u (T3). Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BiCAMS) (Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT); Brief Visuospatial Memory Test (BVMT)).3 |
|
Amato et al. 2014 Computer-assisted rehabilitation of attention in patients with multiple sclerosis: results of a randomized, double-blind trial ItalyRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=102, NFinal=88 |
Population: Specific Computer Training (ST) (n=55): Mean age=40.1yr; Gender: males=11, females=44; Disease course: RRMS=55; Mean EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration=12.0yr.
Non-Specific Computer Training (n-ST) (n=33): Mean age=42.4yr; Gender: males=8, females=25; Disease course: RRMS=33; Mean EDSS=3.0; Mean disease duration=14.7yr. Intervention: Patients were randomized to receive either specific or non-specific computer training in 1-hr sessions, 2x/wk for 3mos. The specific training was home-based Attention Processing Training (APT) that targets selective, sustained, focused, alternating, and divided attention. The non-specific training included reading comprehension exercises and descriptions of pictures. Assessments were made at baseline, after 3mos of treatment, and at 6mos since the beginning of treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Rao's Brief Repeatable Battery (Selective Reminding Test (SRT); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 2 and 3 (PASAT-2, PASAT-3); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Word List Generation Test (WLGT)); Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT); Trail Making Test A and B (TMT-A, TMT-B); The Visual Search: selective attention (VAS).3 |
|
Hildebrandt et al. 2007 Cognitive training in MS: Effects and relation to brain atrophy GermanyRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=42, NFinal=42 |
Population: Intervention group (n=17): Mean age=42.4yr; Gender: males=5, females=12 Disease course: RRMS=17; Mean EDSS=2.9; Mean disease duration=5.4yr.
Control group (n=25): Mean age=36.5yr; Gender: males=12, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=25; Mean EDSS=2.7; Mean disease duration=4.5yr. Intervention: RRMS participants were randomized to either an intervention group or a control group. The intervention group underwent home-based computer cognitive therapy for 6wks. The treatment group used VILAT-G to perform memory and working memory tasks. Cognitive therapy was started at least 4wks after methylprednisolone treatment was discontinued. The control group received no treatment. Assessments were performed before and 2wks after treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale (WAIS): similarities, information; Block design; Picture completion; California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT): short delay free recall, short delay cued recall, long delay free recall, long delay cued recall; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test: 3 second (PASAT); Test of Attentional Performance (TAP): object alternation (OA); TAP: alertness task.3. |
|
Naeeni Davarani et al. 2020 RehaCom rehabilitation training improves a wide range of cognitive functions in multiple sclerosis patients IranRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=60, NFinal=54 |
Population: Intervention group (n=28): Mean age=39.3yr; Sex: males=5, females=23; Disease course: unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified.
Control group (n=26): Mean age=37.55yr; Sex: males=5, females=21; Disease course: unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Following randomization, the intervention group received 2, 60-min sessions/wk for 5wks. The intervention group was assigned the RehaCom software-based intervention. The modules selected for this study were the responsiveness, divided attention 2, attention and concentration, logical reasoning, and spatial operations 3D modules to target working memory, attention, processing speed, executive function, and spatial awareness. The control group received no intervention. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, following the intervention, and at 5-wk and 10-wk f/u. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Integrated Auditory Visual-2 (IVA-2); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Judgement of Line Orientation (JLO); Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS).3 |
|
Arsoy, Tuzun, and Turkoglu 2018 Effects of computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation in benign multiple sclerosis TurkeyRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=81, NFinal=81 |
Population: Benign MS group (n=21 of which n=10 received the intervention and n=11 constituted the Benign MS control group): Mean age=37.0yr; Sex: males=6, females=15; Mean EDSS=2.2; Mean disease duration=13.2yr.
Non-Benign MS control group (n=22): Mean age=39.3yr; Sex: males=10, females=12; Mean EDSS=4.2; Disease course: RRMS=14, SPMS=8; Mean disease duration=14.8yr.
Healthy Control group (n=38): Mean age=36.0yr; Sex: males=10, females=28. Intervention: Benign MS participants (EDSS <3 with disease duration> 10yrs) where randomized to the intervention group (n=10) or a Benign MS control group (n=11). Benign MS participants in the intervention group received the computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation (CCR), which was based on the NOROSOFT Mental Exercise Software and contained five modules. The modules focused on attention, memory, reasoning, visual, and verbal tasks. Participants practiced 5d/wk for 50-min sessions The control group Benign MS participants and other control groups received no intervention. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and at 6mos. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (Selective Reminding Test (SRT), 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3 (PASAT-3), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT)); Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT).3. |
|
Flachenecker et al. 2017 Neuropsychological Training of Attention Improves MS-Related Fatigue: Results of a Randomized, Placebo-Controlled, Double-Blind Pilot Study GermanyRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=32, NFinal=30 |
Population: Intervention group (n=14): Mean age=43.3yr; Sex: males=6, females=8; Disease course: unspecified; Mean EDSS=3.8; Mean disease duration=6.5yr.
Active control group (n=16): Mean age=45.2yr; Sex: males=2, females=14; Disease course: unspecified; Mean EDSS=4.7; Mean disease duration=9.4yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups participated in 2, 30-min training sessions/day for over 5 sessions/wk for 2wks of supervised computerized neuropsychological training in addition to their regular rehabilitation program. The intervention group received computer training with reaction time-based tasks (Reaktion and Jeton software) in which the difficulty of the reaction time-based tasks were individualized to the patient. The active control group received non-reaction time-based computer training with software designed to improve selective attention, cognitive flexibility, and working memory (Bilder, Garten, Mosaik, Partino, and Vario). Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention at 2wks. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Test of Attentional Performance (TAP).1 |
|
Grasso et al. 2017 Evaluation of the Impact of Cognitive Training on Quality of Life in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis ItalyRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=34, NFinal=34 |
Population: Intervention group (n=17): Mean age=59.5yr; Sex: males=6, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=8, PPMS=2, SPMS=7; Mean EDSS=7.54; Mean disease duration=21.64yr.
Control group (n=17): Mean age=58.67yr; Sex: males=6, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=8, PPMS=1, SPMS=8; Mean EDSS=7.5; Mean disease duration=21.9yr. Intervention: Following randomization, all participants received 2 daily physiotherapy sessions for 5d/wk. This treatment was focused on improving movements of paretic upper limb and improving balance and transfers. Both groups also received their respective intervention for 3x/wk for 3mos. The intervention group underwent computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation. This program was based on the Attention Processing Training (APT) program. The tasks were focused on increasing demands on complex attention and working memory. The control group underwent standard occupational therapy instead of the computer-based therapy. Outcome measures were collected 3d after admission, after the 3-mo intervention. and 6mos after the end of treatment Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (Selective Reminding Test (SRT), 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3 (PASAT-3), Word List Generation Test (WLGT)); Stroop Color-Word Interference Test (SCWT)3. |
|
Messinis et al. 2017 Efficacy of a Computer-Assisted Cognitive Rehabilitation Intervention in Relapsing-Remitting Multiple Sclerosis Patients: A Multicenter Randomized Controlled Trial GreeceRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=58, NFinal=58 |
Population: Intervention group (n=32): Mean age=46.03yr; Sex: males=10, females=22; Disease course: RRMS; Median EDSS=3; Mean disease duration=13.31yr.
Control group (n=26): Mean age=45.15yr; Sex: males=8, females=18; Disease course: RRMS; Median EDSS=3.5; Mean disease duration=11.27yr. Intervention: Following randomization, participants in both groups received 2, 60-min sessions/wk for 10wks. The RehaCom software was used for the intervention delivery. The RehaCom program includes 20 modules and adapts the level of difficulty based on the participant's performance. These modules target attention, executive function, information processing, and memory. The control group received their usual clinical care. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention, with one more f/u for the intervention group at 6mos. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R); Selective Reminding Test (SRT); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Trail Making Test Part A (TMT-A); Verbal Fluency Test (VFT); Stroop Colour-Word Task (SCWT); Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B).3 |
|
Perez-Martin et al. 2017 Efficacy of a short cognitive training program in patients with multiple sclerosis SpainRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=62, NFinal=62 |
Population: Treatment group (n=30): Mean age=44.93yr; Gender: males=12, females=18; Disease course: RRMS=27, SPMS=2, PPMS=1; Mean EDSS=2.78; Mean disease duration=11.50yr.
Control group (n=32): Mean age=40.88yr; Gender: males=18, females=14; Disease course: RRMS=30, PPMS=2; Mean EDSS=2.11; Mean disease duration=9.59yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to receive a neuropsychological training program and compensatory strategies or no intervention. The intervention group participated in weekly cognitive training sessions lasting 60-75min each. The exercises focused on attention, processing speed, memory, and executive function, and used a mixture of computer-based exercises and traditional pen-and-paper exercises. Patients were given booklets with exercises and compensatory strategies to complete at home. Assessments were performed at baseline and after 3mos of therapy. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Test (BRB-N)1 (Selective Reminding Test (SRT): long-term storage (SRT-LTS), consistent long-term retrieval (SRT-CLTR), delayed recall (SRT-DR); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART): total, delayed recall (SPART-DR); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 3 seconds (PASAT); Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT): Verbal Fluency FAS Test, animals)); MS neuropsychological questionnaire (MSNQ).2. |
|
Pusswald et al. 2014 A neuropsychological rehabilitation program for patients with Multiple Sclerosis based on the model of the ICF AustriaRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=40, NFinal=40 |
Population: Total population (n=40): Disease course: RRMS=33, SPMS=6, PPMS=1.
Intervention group (n=20): Mean age=42.6yr; Gender: males=5, females=15; Disease course: unspecified; Mean EDSS=3; Mean disease duration=15.1yr.
Control group (n=20): Mean age=45.3yr; Gender: males=4, females=16; Disease course: unspecified; Mean EDSS=4; Mean disease duration=12.6yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to an intervention group or a control group. The intervention group received 30-min sessions 3x/wk of computer-based home training of attention, and 90-min/wk of group psychological counselling, which focused on compensatory strategies, for 5wks. The computer-based training used Fresh minder 2 software. The control group received no training. Assessments were performed at baseline, after 5wks, and after 3mos. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Test of Attentional Performance (TAP): Alertness and Divided Attention; Multiple Sclerosis Inventory for Cognition (MUSIC): verbal memory, verbal retrieval, verbal fluency, interferences.3 |
|
Mattioli et al. 2012 Persistence of the effects of attention and executive functions intensive rehabilitation in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis ItalyRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=24, NFinal=24 |
Population: Intervention group (n=13): Mean age=45.46yr; Gender: Unspecified; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.34; Mean disease duration=16.69yr.
Control group (n=11): Mean age=46.90yr; Gender: Unspecified; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.40; Mean disease duration=20.00yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to the intervention group that received neuropsychological treatment for 3mos or to the control group that received no treatment. Neuropsychological training used the RehaCom software modules for attention, information processing, and executive function exercises. The training consisted of 1-hr sessions 3x/wk for 3 consecutive mos. Assessments were performed at baseline (T0), 3mos (T1), and 9mos after baseline (T2). Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 2, 3 seconds (PASAT-2, -3); Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST) total errors (te), perseverative errors (pe), perseverative responses (pr); Controlled Oral Word Association Test with phonemic (COWA/P) and semantic (COWA/S) cues; Divided attention of Test of Everyday Attention (TEA); Selective Reminding Test (SRT): consistent long-term retrieval (SRT-CLTR), delayed recall (SRT-DR); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test: long term retrieval (10/36 SPART LTR), delayed recall (10/36 SPART LTR); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).3 |
|
Arian Darestani et al. 2020 The therapeutic effect of treatment with RehaCom software on verbal performance in patients with multiple sclerosis IranRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=60, NFinal=53 |
Population: Intervention group (n=27): Mean age=37.11yr; Sex: males=6, females=21; Disease course: unspecified; Mean/Median Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified.
Control group (n=26): Mean age=39.23yr; Sex: males=4, females=22; Disease course: unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Following randomization, participants in the intervention group completed the RehaCom intervention. The intervention included 10, 1-hr sessions over 5wks. RehaCom cognitive rehabilitation software includes 20 modules and adapts the level of difficulty based on the participant's performance. These modules offer attention, memory, executive function, visual field, and visuo-motor training. The control group received no treatment. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, after completion (5wks), and 5wks after completion (10wks). Cognitive Outcome Measures: Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT); California Verbal Learning Test- II (CVLT-II).3 |
|
Rahmani et al. 2020 Comparing the Effectiveness of Computer-Based, Manual-based, and Combined Cognitive Rehabilitation on Cognitive Functions in Relapsing-Remitting Multiple Sclerosis Patients IranRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=60, NFinal=60 |
Population: Computer-Based Intervention group (n=12): Mean age=30.16yr; Sex: female=12; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=2-7yr.
Manual Cognitive Training group (n=12): Mean age= 29.41y; Sex: females=12; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=2-7yr.
Combined Computer and Manual Intervention group (n=12): Mean age=27.83yr; Sex: females=12; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=2-7yr.
Placebo Physical Therapy group (n=12): Mean age=31.16yr; Sex: females=12; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=2-7yr.
Control No Intervention group (n=12): Mean age=29.7yr; Sex: females=12; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=2-7yr. Intervention: The intervention was split up into 3 groups, including the computer-based intervention, the manual intervention, and a combined intervention. The other two groups included the placebo group, which received physical therapy intervention, and the control group, which received no intervention. A total of 21 sessions over 5mos were conducted for the intervention groups using the Pars Cognitive Rehabilitation Package and Captain's Log Computerized Cognitive Training System. This program has exercises for improving attention and working memory. Difficulty of the tasks changed based on the participant's responses. The outcome measures were collected at baseline, end of the intervention, and at 2-mo f/u. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Stroop Colour-Word Test (SCWT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST).3 |
|
Gich et al. 2015 A randomized, controlled, single-blind, 6-month pilot study to evaluate the efficacy of MS-Line!: a cognitive rehabilitation programme for patients with multiple sclerosis SpainRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=43, NFinal=41 |
Population: Experimental group (n=22): Mean age=45.5yr; Gender: males=6, females=16; Disease course: RRMS=21, SPMS=1; Mean EDSS=2.6; Mean disease duration=9.8yr.
Control group (n=22): Mean age=44.0yr; Gender: males=9, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=17, SPMS=4; Mean EDSS=2.8; Mean disease duration=10.7yr. Intervention: MS participants with mild CI were randomized to receive cognitive rehabilitation (experimental group), or to the control condition for 6mos. Participants in the experimental group received 2, 75-min sessions/wk of MS-Line! cognitive rehabilitation for 6mos. MS-Line! consists of written, computer-based, and manipulative (spatial) material. The experimental group also performed short daily cognitive exercises at home lasting no more than 5min. Participants in the control group received no treatment. Assessments were performed at baseline and after 6mos of treatment. Inclusion criteria required CI defined as < -1.5 SD below normative values on cognitive tests. Cognitive Outcome Measures: 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36 SPART): total (T), delayed recall (DR)1; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Word List Generation Test (WLGT); Phonemic Fluency Test (FAS); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-third edition (WAIS-III): digit span, block design, and letter number sequencing (LNS)1; Boston Naming Test (BNT); Trail Making Test-A, -B (TMT-A, -B); Selective Reminding Test (SRT): total (T), long-term storage (LTS), consistent long-term retrieval (CLTR), delayed recall (DR).1 |
|
Hancock et al. 2015 Processing speed and working memory training in multiple sclerosis: a double-blind randomized controlled pilot study USARCT PEDro=5 NInitial=71, NFinal=40 |
Population: Total population: Gender: males=6, females=24; Disease course: RRMS=21, PPMS=4, SPMS=5.
Cognitive training (n=15): Mean age=50.65yr; Gender: Unspecified; Disease course: RRMS=10; Severity: Unspecified; Mean disease duration=128mos.
Sham (n=15): Mean age=49.13yr; Gender: unspecified; Disease course: RRMS=10; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=180mos. Intervention: MS participants were randomized to receive either active or sham computerized cognitive training focused on improving memory speed and working memory. The active group used Posit Science Insight and Brain Twister visual n-back programs in their homes for 30min 6x/wk for 6wks to target working memory and information processing speed. The sham group used the same software on introductory difficulty mode. Outcomes were assessed at baseline and after intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Letter-number sequencing (LNS); Brief Visuospatial Memory Test (BVMT) Trials 1-3; Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT); Conner's Continuous T-score (CPT); Auditory Verbal Learning Task (AVLT) Trials 1-5.3. |
|
Filippi et al. 2012 Multiple sclerosis: effects of cognitive rehabilitation on structural and functional MR Imaging measures—an explorative study ItalyRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: Intervention group (n=10): Mean age=46.7yr; Gender: Female=10; Disease course: RRMS=10; Mean EDSS=2; Mean disease duration=13.5yr.
Control group (n=10): Mean age=44.8yr; Gender: Female=10; Disease course: RRMS=10; Mean EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration=15.5yr. Intervention: RRMS patients were randomized to receive either the intervention or control condition. The intervention group received computer-assisted individual cognitive rehabilitation of attention, information processing, and executive functions in 1-hr sessions, 3x/wk for 12wks using RehaCom software. The RehaCom modules used were “Plan a Day” and “Divided Attention.” The control group received no rehabilitation. Assessments were performed before and 3mos later. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test: 2 second, 3 seconds (PASAT-2, -3); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST): total errors, perseverative responses; Controlled Oral Word Association Test with phonemic cues (COWAT/P); Controlled Oral Word Association Test with semantic cues (COWAT/S); Test of everyday attention (TEA): auditory stimulus, visual stimulus, total omitted stimuli, total errors; Selective Reminding Test for verbal learning/delayed recall (SRT-DR); Selective Reminding Test for verbal learning/consistent long-term retrieval (SRT-CLTR); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test long-term retrieval (10/36 SPART LTR); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test delayed recall (10/36 SPART DR).3 |
|
Tesar, Bandion, and Baumhackl 2005 Efficacy of a neuropsychological training programme for patients with multiple sclerosis -- a randomised controlled trial AustriaRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=19, NFinal=19 |
Population: Treatment group (n=10): Mean age=45.3yr; Gender: males=3, females=7; Disease course: RRMS=7, CPMS=3; Mean EDSS=4.5; Mean disease duration=8yr.
Control group (n=10): Mean age=46.9yr; Gender: males=4, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=6, CPMS=3; Mean EDSS=4.4; Mean disease duration=10.4yr. Intervention: MS participants were randomized into the Rehacom-based neuropsychological training treatment group, or to the control group. The training programme consisted of RehaCom training targeting the two weakest cognitive areas individualized to each patient, as well as the teaching of compensation and relaxation strategies. The compensatory strategies targeted memory and attention and planning. Neuropsychological training consisted of 12, 1-hr sessions over 4wks. Assessments were performed at baseline, immediately after the training programme at 4wks, and at a f/u 3mos later. Patients had mild to moderate CI at baseline. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Computer-aided card-sorting procedure (CKV); Verbal learning test (VLT); Non-verbal learning test (NVLT); Sustained attention test (DAUF): correct, incorrect, reaction time, variation reaction time; Mosaic Test from Hamburg Wechsler Intelligence Test (HAWIE-R).3 |
|
Mattioli et al. 2016 Two years follow up of domain specific cognitive training in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis: A randomized clinical trial ItalyRCT PEDro=4 NInitial=41, NFinal=32 |
Population: Aspecific group (n=17): Mean age=44.88yr; Gender: unspecified; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.97; Mean disease duration=87.18mos.
Specific group (n=15): Mean age=44.80yr; Gender: unspecified; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=1.63; Mean disease duration=67.20mos. Intervention: MS participants were randomized to receive either specific cognitive rehabilitation or to aspecific psychological intervention for 1yr or 2yr if still cognitively impaired. Assessments were performed at baseline (T0), and after 12 and 24mos of treatment (T12 and T24). Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 3, 2 seconds (PASAT-3, -2); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36, 10/36-SPART, SPART): delayed recall (SPART-DR); Selective Reminding Test: Long-term storage (SRT-LTS), Consistent long-term retrieval (SRT-CLTR), Delayed recall (SRT-DR); Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT): Phoneme (P), Category (C); Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT).3 |
|
Olga 2014 Training of attention in patients with remitting-relapsing multiple sclerosis UkraineRCT PEDro=4 NInitial=20, NFinal=19 |
Population: Intervention group (n=13): Disease course: RRMS. No further information provided.
Control group (n=7): Disease course: RRMS. No further information provided Intervention: Participants were randomized to either the intervention group, who used the computer-based cognitive rehabilitation program ERICA, or to the control group, who were asked to complete paper and pencil tasks (e.g., “find 10 differences”). The ERICA modules targeted focused and selective attention. The groups trained for 40min/d for 3wks. Assessment was performed at baseline and at 3wks. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Leiter-3 test: concentration, attention, Stroop Test (SCWT).3. |
|
Shatil et al. 2010 Home-based personalized cognitive training in MS patients: a study of adherence and cognitive performance IsraelRCT PEDro=3 NInitial=107, NFinal=46 |
Population: Training group (n=59): Mean age=43.78yr; Gender: males=15, females=44; Disease course: RRMS, PRMS; Mean EDSS=3.06; Disease duration: Unspecified.
Control group (n=48): Mean age=41.35yr; Gender: males=9, females=39; Disease course: RRMS, PRMS; Mean EDSS=2.66; Disease duration: Unspecified. Intervention: MS participants were randomized to a cognitive training program or to a control condition. The training group was instructed to train 3x/ wk for 12wks with the CogniFit Personal Coach program. This program has 21 different training tasks that target 17 cognitive abilities, but the study did not specify what these tasks or targets were. The control group received no training. Outcomes were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Neuropsychological Examination – CogniFit Personal Coach (N-CPC): Auditory working memory, Awareness, Divided attention, Avoiding distractions, Hand-eye coordination, General memory, Inhibition, Naming, Planning, Response time, Shifting attention, Spatial perception, Time estimation, Visual working memory, Visual perception, Visual scanning, and Verbal auditory working memory; Memory tasks: Flowers and numbers: visuo-spatial working memory, digit forward, digit backward; Letters: naming speed, naming accuracy; Pictures and words: working memory reaction time, working memory accuracy; Television: working memory accuracy; Objects seen or heard before: recall speed, recall accuracy.3 |
|
Hubacher et al. 2015 Case-based fMRI analysis after cognitive rehabilitation in MS: A novel approach SwitzerlandRCT PEDro=1 NInitial=16, NFinal=10 |
Population: Treatment group (n=6): Mean age=47.5yr; Gender: males=2, females=4; Disease course: RRMS=6; Mean EDSS=2.42; Mean disease duration=2.5yr.
Control group (n=4): Mean age=44.75yr; Gender: males=3, females=1; Disease course: RRMS=4; Mean EDSS=1.63; Mean disease duration=2.25yr. Intervention: RRMS participants were randomized to the training or control groups. The treatment group received a 4-wk computerized working memory training, consisting of 16 sessions, 45min/session. The control group received no intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Corsi block backwards task; Digit span backwards test; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Test of Attentional Performance (TAP): alertness task (tonic and phasic).3. |
|
Barbarulo et al. 2018 Integrated Cognitive and Neuromotor Rehabilitation in Multiple Sclerosis: A Pragmatic Study ItalyPCT NInitial=63, NFinal=63 |
Population: Intervention group (n=32): Mean age=50.22yr; Sex: males=12, females=20; Disease course: RRMS=4, Progressive MS=28; Mean EDSS=5.76; Mean disease duration=17.7yr.
Control group (n=31): Mean age=46.2yr; Sex: males=13, females=18; Disease course: RRMS=5, Progressive MS: 26; Mean EDSS=5.16; Mean disease duration=17.2yr. Intervention: Participants were assigned to either the intervention group, who completed both neuropsychological treatment and conventional neuromotor rehabilitation, or the control group, who received only neuromotor rehabilitation. Both groups received 2, 60-min sessions/wk for 24wks. The treatment group completed the ERICA software and paper-pencil task, complemented by conventional therapy. The focus was on exercises for attention, spatial cognition, memory, and verbal and non-verbal executive function. Exercise complexity was individualized to each patient. The neuromotor rehabilitation included individualized balance and gait exercises and was progressed based on the individual participant's needs. Pelvic floor dysfunction exercises were also prescribed. All participants were cognitively impaired, defined as 1.5 SD below normative values. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention at 24wks. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Rao's Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB) (Selective Reminding (SRT), 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT), Word List Generation Test (WLGT)); Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT); Forward and Backward Verbal Span; Spatial Span; Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB); Raven's Coloured Metrics (RCMPs); Phonological Verbal Fluency Task (PFV).3 |
|
Bonavita et al. 2015 Computer-aided cognitive rehabilitation improves cognitive performances and induces brain functional connectivity changes in relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis patients: an exploratory study ItalyPCT reporting pre-post results NInitial=32, NFinal=32 |
Population: Computer-based Cognitive Therapy (cCR) (n=18): Mean age=49yr; Gender: males=0, females=18; Disease course: RRMS=18, Mean EDSS=5; Mean disease duration=22yr. aspecific Cognitive Therapy (aCR) (n=14); Mean age=46yr; Gender: males=1, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=14, Mean EDSS=4; Mean disease duration=21yr. Intervention: The cCR group received computer-based cognitive rehabilitation 2x/wk for 8wks using RehaCom software. The RehaCom modules used were attention and concentration, plan a day, divided attention, reaction behavior, and logical thinking. These modules intend to target attention, information processing, and executive function. The aCR group received aspecific cognitive rehabilitation consisting of reading a newspaper for about 30min, 2x/wk for 8wks, and subsequently explaining the content read to a resident in neurology. Assessments were measured at baseline and after treatment (8wks). To avoid training effects, version A of the Brief Repeatable Battery (BRB) was used at baseline and version B was used after aCR or cCR. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery (BRB) (Selective Reminding Test (SRT); SRT-delayed recall (SRT-D); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART); 10/36 SPART-delayed recall (SPART-D)); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT-3, PASAT-2); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Word List Generation Test (WLGT); Stroop Color-Word Interference Test (SCWIT).3. |
|
Vogt et al. 2009 Working memory training in patients with multiple sclerosis - comparison of two different training schedules SwitzerlandPCT NInitial=45, NFinal=45 |
Population: High-Intensity group (n=15): Mean age=43.20yr; Gender: males=4, females=11; Mean EDSS=3.23; Mean disease duration=9.13yr. Distributed group (n=15): Mean age=43.40yr; Gender: males=6, females=9; Mean EDSS=2.3; Mean disease duration=8.13yr. Control group (n=15): Mean age=46.27yr; Gender: males=5, females=10; Mean EDSS=3.20; Mean disease duration=12.06yr. Intervention: MS patients were allocated to two different computer-based working memory training groups (using BrainStim) and a control group without training. The High-Intensity training group received 16 training sessions 4x/wk for 4wks. The Distributed training group trained 2x/wk for 8wks. Assessments were performed at baseline and after training. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Corsi Blocks forward and backward; Wechsler Memory Scale revised (WMS-R): Digit span forward and backward; Test of Attentional Performance (TAP): 2-back numbers correct, omissions, reaction time; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Faces Symbol Test (FST); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).3 |
|
Mendozzi et al. 1998 Computer-assisted memory retraining of patients with multiple sclerosis ItalyPCT NInitial=60, NFinal=59 |
Population: Specific group (n=20): Mean age=47.92yr; Gender: males=9, females=11; Disease course: RRMS, SPMS; Mean EDSS=3.65; Mean disease duration=12.00yr. Non-specific group (n=20): Mean age=45.92yr; Gender: males=8, females=12; Disease course: RRMS, SPMS; Mean EDSS=4.00; Mean disease duration=10.70yr. Control group (n=20): Mean age=45.38yr; Gender: males=10, females=10; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3.30; Mean disease duration=10.15yr. Intervention: MS participants received either a specific computer-assisted memory retraining (SCRP) programme, a non-specific computer-based cognitive rehabilitation programme (NCRP), or no treatment. Cognitive training for the specific and non-specific groups consisted of 15, ~45-min sessions 2x/wk for an average duration of 8wks, and employed Rehacom procedures for cognitive rehabilitation. The SCRP group focused on attention and memory while the NCRP group focused on reaction time and visual tracking. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS): story recall, digit span, visual production, verbal paired associates; Corsi test: spatial span (CORSI); Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery (LNNB): memory scale; Signal Detection Task: number of hits, reaction time.3 |
|
Plohmann, Kappos, and Brunnschweiler 1994 Evaluation of a computer-based attention retraining program for patients with multiple sclerosis SwitzerlandPCT NInitial=10, NFinal=10 |
Population: Mean age=39.7yr; Gender: males=2, females=8; Disease course: SPMS=5, RRMS=5; Mean EDSS=3.95; Mean disease duration=9.5yr. Intervention: Participants were divided into pairs. Half of the participants (one of each pair) were matched to the waitlist control and the other participants began the computer-based attention-retraining program. The program constituted 16 training sessions, 4x/wk, 45-60min/session. Comprehensive neuropsychological testing including the attention test battery (TAP) were administered prior to and following the training program. TAP testing was also repeated at 3mos post training. When the first half of participants completed training, the second half began the training program. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Attention Test Battery (TAP); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); 7/24 Spatial Recall Test. |
|
Sharifi, Yazdanbakhsh, and Momeni 2019 The Effectiveness of Computer-Based Cognitive Rehabilitation in Executive Functions in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis IranControlled Quasi Experimental NInitial=20, NFinal=not reported |
Population: Intervention group (n=10): Mean age=38.10yr; Sex: males=4, females=6; Disease course: unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified. Control group (n=10): Mean age=36yr; Sex: males=5, females=5; Disease course: unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Twenty of 60 participants with less-impaired executive function on the computerized executive function perseverative test (score ≤20) were randomized to the experimental or control groups. The intervention group completed 12, 50-min sessions over 6wks. The intervention used the Captain's Log software and specifically focused on the executive functions module. This module included stimulus reaction/inhibition and scanning reaction/inhibition training. As the participants progressed through the 15 stages, the tasks would become more difficult. The control group did not receive any treatment. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)1. |
|
Bonzano et al. 2020 Brain activity pattern changes after adaptive working memory training in multiple sclerosis ItalyPre-post NInitial=36, NFinal=36 |
Population: MS group (n=18): Mean age=45.3yr; Sex: males=6, females=12; Disease course: RRMS=12, SPMS=6; Median EDSS=3.5; Mean disease duration=14.3yr. Healthy subjects (n=18): Mean age=41.6yr; Sex: males=8, females=10. Intervention: : Participants with MS completed 5, 30-min sessions/wk for 8ks of the self-administered, at-home, computer-based rehabilitation protocol. Each session included three types of working memory training: a visuospatial task, an operation N-back task and a dual N-back task. Task difficulty was adjusted based on the number of correct and incorrect responses. Outcome and fMRI measures were completed at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (Selective Reminding Test (SRT), 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Word List Generation Test (WLGT)).3 |
|
Fuchs et al. 2020 Functional Connectivity and Structural Disruption in the Default-Mode Network Predicts Cognitive Rehabilitation Outcomes in Multiple Sclerosis USAPre-post NInitial=25, NFinal=X |
Population:Mean age=54.7yr; Sex: males=4, females=21; Disease course: RRMS=19, PMS=6; Median EDSS=4; Mean disease duration=23.5yr. Intervention: Participants completed 60, 45-60-min sessions over 12wks. The participants are a subgroup of the Fuchs et al. (2019) study. The participants used the BrainHQ program for their intervention and the training focused on improving cognitive processing speed. Baseline MRI testing was completed between 1.6-2.8 years prior to baseline neuropsychological testing. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).1. |
|
Barker et al. 2019 A Pilot Study to Assess At-Home Speed of Processing Training for Individuals with Multiple Sclerosis USAPre-post NInitial=15, NFinal=12 |
Population: Mean age=47.0yr; Sex: males=3, females=12; Disease course: unspecified; Median EDSS=1.75; Mean disease duration=14.4yr. Intervention: Participants were enrolled in an at-home computerized Speed of Processing (SOP) training intervention using the Visual Rehabilitation package of BrainHQ. The intervention included 10 sessions, 2x/wk for 15wks. The intervention involved five major tasks. While the tasks changed slightly, they all shared the same visual-based speed component. Task difficulty was automatically adjusted once the user maintained an 85% correct rate. Outcome measures were completed at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (Selective Reminding (SRT), 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT)); Stroop Test (SCWT).3 |
|
Fuchs et al. 2019 Response heterogeneity to home-based restorative cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis: An exploratory study USAPre-post NInitial=54, NFinal=51 |
Population: Mean age=56.1yr; Sex: males=15, females=36; Disease course: RRMS=35, PPMS=4, SPMS=12; Median EDSS=4.0; Mean disease duration=21.6yr. Intervention: Participants completed 60, 45-60-min sessions over 12wks. The participants used the BrainHQ program for their intervention and the training focused on improving cognitive processing speed. Participants were contacted 1x/wk for technical support and mild encouragement. Baseline MRI was completed 2yr before baseline testing. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment for Multiple Sclerosis (BiCAMS) (Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)1, California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II)2, Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)2; Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System (D-KEFS).2 |
|
Covey et al. 2018 Improved cognitive performance and event-related potential changes following working memory training in patients with multiple sclerosis USAPre-post NInitial=24, NFinal=24 |
Population: Intervention group (n=12): Mean age=32.83yr; Sex: males=4, females=8; Disease course: RRMS; Median EDSS=2; Mean disease duration=9.17yr.
Healthy Control group (n=12): Mean age=26.25yr; Sex: males=3, females=9. Intervention: Participants completed 20, 25-30-min sessions, 5d/wk for 4wks. The sessions targeted working memory by using n-back training, during which a series of letters were presented on the screen and the participants had to actively recall if a certain letter was presented n trials back. Difficulty was adjusted based on performance of each trial. An average of 10 blocks per session was used to obtain an index of performance for the session, which was then used as a measure of improvement on the training task. Outcome measures and electrophysiology recordings were completed at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Raven's Advanced Progressive Metrices (RAPM) shortened version; Analysis Synthesis and Concept Formation WJ-R Tests; Letter Series.3 |
|
Altun et al. 2015 The effects of cognitive rehabilitation on relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis patients TurkeyPre-Post NInitial=32, NFinal=32 |
Population: Mean age=36.09yr; Gender: males=8, females=24; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.08; Mean disease duration=9.31yr. Intervention: RRMS participants received computer-assisted cognitive rehabilitation programs for developing coping strategies and improving attention, visual and verbal memory, and information processing speed 1x/wk for 8wks. Assessments were performed at baseline and after 4 and 8wks of treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Selective Reminding Test-total learning (SRT-TL); Selective Reminding Test-long-term memory (SRT-DR); Spatial recall test-total learning (SPART-TL); Spatial recall test-long-term memory (SPART-DR); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Word List Generation (WLG)); MS neuropsychological screening questionnaire (MSNQ).3 |
|
Sastre-Garriga et al. 2011 A functional magnetic resonance proof of concept pilot trial of cognitive rehabilitation in multiple sclerosis SpainPre-Post NInitial=15, NFinal=15 |
Population: MS participants (n=15): Mean age=50.73yr; Gender: males=5, females=10; Disease course: RRMS=3, PPMS=2, SPMS=10; Mean EDSS=6.0; Mean disease duration=14.43yr.
Healthy participants (n=5). Intervention: Participants underwent a 5-wk cognitive rehabilitation program, using computer and non-computer exercises aimed at speed of information processing, attention, executive functions, memory, and high-level language functions. Participants completed 3 weekly sessions. Outcome measures were collected 5wks prior to start of the program, at the beginning of the program, and following the program. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Digit span (DS) forward; Digit span (DS) backward; Digit span (sum); Trail Making Test A, B (TMT-A, -B); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).3 |
|
Allen et al. 1998 Teaching memory strategies to persons with multiple sclerosis USAPre-Post NInitial=10, NFinal=8 |
Population: Mean age=39.6yr; Mean EDSS=4.0. No further information provided. Intervention: MS participants took part in a memory-training program that involved computer-based teaching of imagery-based mnemonic strategies for recall of lengthy lists of words and for associating names with faces. The list-learning training involved a story that was presented with 20 words to be remembered printed in bold face. The face-name training coached the participants to associate names with either physical characteristics of pictures of people, or with the person's resemblance to an acquaintance or a celebrity. Participants were assessed on word and name recall immediately after the session. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Number of correct words at free recall; number of correct names after cuing; Memory questionnaires. |
|
Plohmann et al. 1998 Computer assisted retraining of attentional impairments in patients with multiple sclerosis SwitzerlandPre-Post NInitial=22, NFinal=22 |
Population: Mean age=44.6yr; Gender: males=18, females=9; Disease course: RRMS=72.7%, PPMS=4.6%, SPMS=22.7%; Mean EDSS=3.8; Mean disease duration=16.6yr. Intervention: All participants received computer-based treatment on their two most impaired attention functions using AIXTENT software. The four attention domains AIXTENT can target are: alertness, sustained attention, selective attention, and divided attention. Each participant was trained on one function at a time, each training period lasting 12 sessions over 3wks. In each treatment period, there was one attention domain trained specifically and at least one more function that was trained in a non-specific manner. Thus, participants were trained on one of their weak areas of attention for 12wks, and then on another weak area of attention for another 12 wks. Participants were assessed three times (T1, T2, T3) at 3-wk intervals before the start of the treatment, immediately after the first training period (T4), immediately after the second treatment period (T5), and twice more post-treatment at 3-wk intervals (T6, T7). Cognitive Outcome Measures: Test of Attentional Performance (TAP): alertness (simple, cued), divided attention, selective attention (go/no-go, incompatibility, flexibility), vigilance.3 |
|
Summary
Table 5. Summary Table of Studies Examining Computer-based Training
Improve | No statistical sig. difference | |
General Cognition/Composite Scores |
|
|
Attention |
|
|
Executive Function |
|
|
Information Processing |
|
|
Memory |
|
|
Verbal Language Skills |
|
|
Visuospatial skills |
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (pre-post) |
Discussion
A growing number of studies in PwMS support that computer training improves cognition on objective outcome measures evaluating the cognitive skills or domains challenged by the training. The research in computer-based cognitive rehabilitation includes variability in the training approaches, comparator groups, dose, intensity, degree of supervision, follow-up period, and baseline cognitive impairment levels. There may be commercial interest in the development of computer-based training products, introducing potential for bias in the conduct of the research.
However, cognitive domain-specific training for processing speed (7 RCTs), memory (6 RCTs), and executive function (7 RCTs) improved cognition on objective testing addressing each of these respective domains (see respective Level of Evidence statements below). The evidence was more conflicting for the verbal language and attention domains (Arian Darestani et al. 2020; Arsoy, Tuzun, and Turkoglu 2018; Filippi et al. 2012; Mäntynen et al. 2014; Flavia Mattioli et al. 2010; F. Mattioli et al. 2012; Pusswald et al. 2014; Alexa K Stuifbergen et al. 2012; A. K. Stuifbergen et al. 2018 and Amato et al. 2014; Campbell et al. 2016; Cerasa et al. 2013; Filippi et al. 2012; Flachenecker et al. 2017; Flavia Mattioli et al. 2010; F. Mattioli et al. 2012; Messinis et al. 2017; Olga 2014; A. M. Plohmann et al. 1998; Rahmani et al. 2020). No studies targeted only verbal language skill computer-based training, although some protocols included verbal language tasks. For attention training, task-specific computer training targeting reaction time specifically improved reaction time on the Test for Attentional Performance (Flachenecker et al. 2017).
Computer-based training may help to maintain current cognitive function or delay progression of cognitive impairment, even if improvements are not realized. Arsoy et al. (2018) studied a small sample of participants with a more benign MS course (EDSS≤3 ten years after MS onset) with mild CI at baseline. Participants received either no intervention or computer-based training five days a week for 50 minutes with the NOROSOFT Mental Exercise Program. The program involved training in attention, memory, reasoning, visual, and verbal tasks. The intervention group maintained stable cognitive testing scores across multiple cognitive domains, and experienced improved scores only on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test. However, the control group significantly worsened on the SDMT and Stroop Test over the six-month period (Arsoy, Tuzun, and Turkoglu 2018). Overall, there are more positive than negative studies supporting that PwMS with minimal CI at baseline at least maintain cognitive test scores with computer training in comparison to no treatment.
A 2019 meta-analysis of 20 RCTs similarly concluded an overall modest effect size for computer-based rehabilitation approaches benefiting cognition. However, the results for working memory, fatigue, and psychosocial and daily functioning were inconclusive. There may exist a minimum dose effect with individual variability in the response to computer cognitive training (Lampit et al. 2019). We analyzed for a possible dose effect for each cognitive domain by totaling the time spent doing computer cognitive training over the duration of each RCT. In trials where the total cognitive training hours exceeded 33 hours (n=4 RCTs), results were consistently positive for outcomes related to the verbal language domain (Arsoy, Tuzun, and Turkoglu 2018; Filippi et al. 2012; Flavia Mattioli et al. 2010; F. Mattioli et al. 2012). These positive results were observed even though the training protocols within these four trials were not specifically targeting verbal language skills. For trials with less than 33 hours of training (n= 4 RCTs), the evidence was conflicting for positive outcomes in the verbal language domain (Mäntynen et al. 2014; Barker et al. 2019; Alexa K Stuifbergen et al. 2012; Arian Darestani et al. 2020). Minimum dose effects, as well as the intensity of the training to achieve positive outcomes across cognitive domains, warrant consideration.
One study reported on brain functional reserve according to baseline white matter tract connectivity on functional MRI (Fuchs, Ziccardi, et al. 2020). Increased baseline white matter tract connectivity was associated with improvement on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test after processing speed training on the BrainHQ computer platform (Fuchs et al. 2020). In clinical practice, some patients self-report they enjoy computer-based training or gaming, while others may find the tasks frustrating. Possibly, a baseline connectivity threshold exists, where if sufficient connections are lost, improvement is more challenging to achieve. Participants with severe CI are not included in the computer-based cognitive intervention MS trials. In the moderate to severe brain injury population, computer-based cognitive training did not provide benefit for improving attention and concentration outcomes (ERABI, n.d.).
The distinction between gaming versus a non-gaming-based computer training protocol is not always clear in the literature. This distinction may be important, since patient expectations and experiences with gaming versus cognitive training paradigms may differ. Three RCTs compared no treatment to what authors describe as computer gaming (De Giglio et al. 2015; 2016; Janssen et al. 2015). The study by Janssen et al. involved a Space Fortress video game where the player shoots at a fortress while avoiding enemies, thus challenging visualspatial skills. Visuospatial memory improved compared to no treatment, yet verbal learning and memory did not (Janssen et al. 2015). Two of the RCTs included Nintendo's Brain Training video games as the intervention, and executive function and information processing speed improved compared to no treatment (De Giglio et al. 2015; 2016). In these studies, the gaming tasks had a similar focus and resemblance to the tests administered as outcome measures, supporting task-specific training effects (De Giglio et al. 2015; 2016).
In a study by Stuifbergen et al. (2018), the intervention group received luminosity training plus additional training in compensatory strategies, while the control group had unsupervised, free access to MyBrainGames (Stuifbergen et al. 2018). Interestingly, both groups improved on cognitive outcomes, with no between-group differences. Charvet et al. (2015) reported improved cognitive outcomes for an intervention group trained on luminosity in comparison to a control group receiving computer software Hoyle puzzle board games (Charvet et al. 2015). When comparing computer HQBrain training to traditional board games, Charvet et al. (2017) found similarly significantly greater improvements in cognition in the HQBrain training group (on the Neuropsychological Composite Score outcome) (Charvet et al. 2017). Computer gaming has the advantage of individualizing the level of difficulty for the participant in real time. This may be harder to achieve with board-based games or word puzzles. However, which computer training protocols are most effective for which cognitive outcomes, and how much oversight might be required to improve or maintain cognitive function, remains unclear.
Computer-based cognitive training programs relying on intact vision and hand function may not be appropriate for some PwMS, and computer cognitive skills training may not be applicable to every-day life. The RehaCom computer training program has been adapted specifically for people with visual and dexterity impairments. Computer-based training is feasible to deliver remotely in one's own home and can be combined with other compensatory or restorative strategies. The added benefit of combining computer training with non-computer-based compensatory strategies (i.e., the use of external memory aids) or restorative approaches also remains unclear (Gich et al. 2015; Perez-Martin et al. 2017; Rahmani et al. 2020; Rodgers et al. 1996; Sastre-Garriga et al. 2011). Encouragingly, there is strong evidence that computer training in processing speed, memory, and executive function improves cognitive performance on testing related to these domains. However, there is less evidence for possible carryover effects across cognitive domains (Plohmann et al. 1998) or into cognitive functioning in life activities. Virtual training or gaming platforms, covered in section 3.5 of this module, may hold even greater promise for benefiting cognition relevant to everyday function.
Conclusion
Attention
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves attention in persons with MS (seventeen randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Amato et al. 2014, Campbell et al. 2016, Cerasa et al. 2013, Filippi et al. 2012, Flachenecker et al. 2017, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Messinis et al. 2017, Orel 2014, Plohmann et al. 1998, Rahmani et al. 2020, and Tesar et al. 2005).
RehaCom vs no treatment
There is conflicting evidence whether RehaCom improves attention in persons with MS with cognitive impairment compared to no treatment (six randomized controlled trials and one prospective controlled trial; Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Mendozzi et al. 1998, Messinis et al. 2017, Naeeni Davarni et al. 2020, and Tesar et al. 2005).
Freshminder 2
There is level 1b evidence that Freshminder 2 combined with counseling for compensatory strategies may improve attention in persons with MS compared to no treatment (one randomized controlled trial; Pusswald et al. 2014).
VILAT-G
There is level 1b evidence that VILAT-G may not improve attention in persons with MS compared to no treatment (one randomized controlled trial; Shatil et al. 2010).
CogniFit 2
There is level 2 evidence that CogniFit 2 may not improve attention in persons with MS compared to no treatment (one randomized controlled trial; Shatil et al. 2010).
RehaCom vs visuomotor control
There is level 1b evidence that RehaCom improves attention more than computer-based visuomotor training in persons with MS with cognitive impairment (one randomized controlled trial; Cerasa et al. 2016).
Computer-based vs pen and paper-based cog rehab training
There is level 2 evidence that ERICA attention exercises may improve attention in persons with MS with cognitive impairment compared to pen-and-paper attention exercises (one randomized controlled trial; Orel et al. 2014).
Target reaction time vs target selective attention/memory/executive function
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation targeting reaction time may improve reaction time more than computer-based cognitive rehabilitation targeting selective attention, working memory, and executive function in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Flachenecker et al. 2017).
Attention-specific training vs non-specific cognitive rehab
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based Attention Processing Training (APT) may not improve all attention domains compared to non-specific cognitive exercises in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Amato et al. 2014).
Executive Function
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves executive function in persons with MS (sixteen randomized controlled trials and two prospective controlled trials; Amato et al. 2014, Arsoy et al. 2018, Bonavita et al. 2015, Cerasa et al. 2013, De Giglio et al. 2015, De Giglio et al. 2016, Filippi et al. 2012, Grasso et al. 2017, Hancock et al. 2015, Mäntynen et al. 2014, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Naeeni Davarani et al. 2020, Rahmani et al. 2020, Sharifi et al. 2019, Stuifbergen et al. 2012, and Tesar et al. 2005).
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation that targets executive function improves executive function compared to no treatment (seven randomized controlled trials and one prospective controlled trial; De Giglio et al. 2015, De Giglio et al. 2016, Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Naeeni Davarani et al. 2020, Sharifi et al. 2019, and Tesar et al. 2005).
RehaCom vs no treatment
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom modules that target executive function improves executive function for persons with MS with cognitive impairment compared to no treatment (four randomized controlled trials; Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, and Tesar et al. 2015)
There is conflicting evidence whether using RehaCom for 8 weeks or less improves executive function for persons with MS with cognitive impairment compared to no treatment or non-specific treatment (one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Bonavita et al. 2015 and Tesar et al. 2015).
RehaCom vs computer-based visuomotor tasks
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom for 60 minutes per day 2 days per week for 6 weeks may improve executive function in persons with MS with cognitive impairmentcompared to computer-based visuomotor tasks (one randomized controlled trial; Cerasa et al. 2013).
MAPSS-MS (Lumosity + neuropsychonline + group therapy for compensatory strategies) vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that the MAPSS-MS program, which combines Lumosity for 45 minutes per day 3 times per week for 8 weeks with group therapy for compensatory strategies, may not improve executive function compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitive impairment (one randomized controlled trial; Stuifbergen et al. 2012).
Captain's Log vs No Treatment
There is level 2 evidence that computer-based rehabilitation using Captain's Log software for 6 weeks may improve executive function in persons with MS compared to no treatment (one prospective controlled trial; Sharifi et al. 2019).
NOROSOFT Mental Exercise Software vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based rehabilitation using NOROSOFT Mental Exercise Software for 24 weeks may help maintain executive function in cognitively impaired persons with MS compared to no treatment (one randomized controlled trial; Arsoy et al. 2018).
Information Processing
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation that specifically targets information processing speed does improve information processing speed in persons with MS compared to no treatment or non-specific cognitive rehabilitation (seven randomized controlled trials, one prospective controlled trial, and four pre-post studies; Barker et al. 2019, Bonavita et al. 2015, Chiaravalloti et al. 2018, Filippi et al. 2012, Fuchs et al. 2019, Fuchs et al. 2020, Guclu Altun et al. 2015, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Messinis et al. 2017, and Messinis et al. 2020, and Rahmani et al. 2020).
RehaCom
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom that specifically targets information processing speed does improve information processing speed in persons with MS with cognitive impairment compared to no treatment or standard MS rehabilitation (four randomized controlled trials and one prospective controlled trial; Bonavita et al. 2015, Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Messinis et al. 2017, and Messinis et al. 2020).
Speed of Processing Training
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using Speed of Processing Training may improve information processing speed in persons with MS with cognitive impairment compared to no treatment (one randomized controlled trial and three pre-post study; Barker et al. 2019, Chiaravalloti et al. 2018, Fuchs et al. 2019, and Fuchs et al. 2020).
ERICA vs traditional cognitive rehab
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using ERICA software may improve visual information processing speed but not auditory information processing speed more than non-computer cognitive rehabilitation approaches in persons with MS with cognitive impairment (one randomized controlled trial; De Luca et al. 2019).
VILAT-G vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using VILAT-G software may improve information processing speed more than no treatment in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Hildebrandt et al. 2007).
Lumosity vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using Lumosity in the MAPSS-MS program may not improve information processing speed compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitive impairment (one randomized controlled trial; Stuifbergen et al. 2012).
Memory
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves memory in persons with MS (18 randomized controlled trials, 6 prospective controlled trials and 1 pre-post study; Amato et al. 2014, Arian Darestani et al. 2020, Arsoy et al. 2018, Barker et al. 2019, Bonavita et al. 2015, Bonzano et al. 2020, Bove et al. 2021, Campbell et al. 2016, Cerasa et al. 2013, Chiaravalloti et al. 2018, Covey et al. 2018, De Luca et al. 2019, Filippi et al. 2012, Fuchs et al. 2019, Hildebrandt et al. 2007, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Mendozzi et al. 1998, Messinis et al. 2017, Messinis et al. 2020, Shatil et al. 2010, Solari et al. 2004, Vogt et al. 2009, and Allen et al., 2018).
Target Memory vs no treatment/nonspecific/usual care
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation that specifically targets memory improves memory in persons with MS compared to no treatment or their usual clinical care (six randomized controlled trials, two prospective controlled trials, and two pre-post studies; Arian Darestani et al. 2020, Bonzano et al. 2020, Covey et al. 2018, Hildebrandt et al. 2007, Janssen et al. 2015, Mendozzi et al. 1998, Messinis et al. 2017, Messinis et al. 2020, Rahmani et al. 2020, Shatil et al. 2010, and Stuifbergen et al. 2012, and Vogt et al. 2009).
RehaCom vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation with RehaCom targeting memory training may improve memory (two randomized controlled trials and one prospective controlled trial; Arian Darestani et al. 2020, Mendozzi et al. 1998, and Messinis et al. 2017).
RehaCom vs watching natural history DVDs or nonspecific computer exercises)
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation with RehaCom targeting memory training improves memory in persons with MS with cognitive impairment compared to natural history DVDs or nonspecific computer exercises (two randomized controlled trials; Campbell et al. 2016 and Messinis et al. 2020).
Norosoft vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using NOROSOFT may not improve memory in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Arsoy et al. 2018).
VILAT-G vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using VILAT-G to specifically target memory may improve memory in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Hildebrandt et al. 2007).
CogniFit vs no treatment
There is level 2 evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using CogniFit to specifically target memory may improve memory in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Hildebrandt et al. 2007).
MAPSS-MS (Lumosity + neuropsychonline + group compensatory strategies) vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using Lumosity to specifically target memory combined with group compensatory strategies may improve memory in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Stuifbergen et al. 2012).
Lumosity + group compensatory vs MyBrainGames
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using Lumosity to specifically target memory combined with group compensatory strategies may not improve memory more than MyBrainGames on multiplesclerosis.com (one randomized controlled trial; Stuifbergen et al. 2018).
ERICA – vs traditional cognitive rehabilitation
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using ERICA to specifically target memory improves spatial memory but not verbal learning and memory more than traditional cognitive rehabilitation (one randomized controlled trial; De Luca et al. 2019).
Verbal Language Skills
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves verbal language skills in persons with MS (nine randomized controlled trials; Arian Darestani et al. 2020, Arsoy et al. 2018, Filippi et al. 2012, Mäntynen et al. 2014, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Pusswald et al. 2014, Stuifbergen et al. 2012, and Stuifbergen et al. 2018).
Computer-based over 33 hours vs no treatment
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation delivered for 33 hours or longer over at least twelve weeks improves verbal language skills compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitive impairment (four randomized controlled trials; Arsoy et al. 2018, Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, and Mattioli et al. 2012).
Computer-based less than 33 hours vs no treatment
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation delivered for less than 33 hours of total training is more effective than no treatment (four randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Barker et al. 2019, Stuifbergen et al. 2012, Mäntynen et al. 2014, Arian Darestani et al. 2020, and Pusswald et al. 2014).
RehaCom 33 hours vs no treatment
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom delivered for 33 hours or longer over twelve weeks improves verbal language skills compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitive impairment (three randomized controlled trials; Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, and Mattioli et al. 2012).
NOROSOFT 100 hours vs no treatment
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using NOROSOFT delivered for 100 hours over twenty-four weeks maintains verbal language skills compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitive impairment (one randomized controlled trial; Arsoy et al. 2018).
Lay Summary
Computer cognitive training in memory improves memory in persons with MS with mild cognitive impairment compared to no treatment.
Computer cognitive training in processing speed improves processing speed in persons with MS with mild cognitive impairment compared to no treatment.
Computer cognitive training in executive function improves executive function in persons with MS with mild cognitive impairment compared to no treatment.
Computer cognitive training in attention may improve attention in persons with MS with mild cognitive impairment compared to no treatment.
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves verbal language skills in persons with MS with minimal cognitive impairment compared to no treatment.
There is conflicting evidence whether the combination of computer-based cognitive rehabilitation with compensatory rehabilitation approaches provides added benefit for improving attention, information processing speed, executive function, spatial skills, verbal language skills, or memory in persons with MS.
Video games are introduced together with computer rehabilitation approaches. Please see section 3.2.
Table 6. Studies Examining Video Games for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
De Giglio et al. 2016 Multiple sclerosis: Changes in thalamic resting-state functional connectivity Induced by a homebased cognitive rehabilitation program ItalyRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=24, NFinal=24 |
Population: Intervention group (n=12): Mean age=43.7yr; Gender: males=4, females=8; Disease course: RRMS=12; Mean EDSS=2; Mean disease duration=12.9yr.
Control group (n=12): Mean age=40.2yr; Gender: males=6, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=12; Mean EDSS=2; Mean disease duration=13.0yr.
Intervention: MS patients with CI were randomized to receive either a video game-based cognitive rehabilitation program or to the control condition (waitlist). The intervention group were trained on video games of memory, attention, visual-spatial processing, and calculation in 30-min sessions, 5d/wk for 8wks. The cognitive training was performed at home with Dr. Kawashima's Brain Training game. Assessments were performed at baseline (T0) and after 8wks of treatment (T1). Cognitive Outcome Measures: Stroop Test (SCWT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT): 3 second; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).3 |
|
De Giglio et al. 2015 A low-cost cognitive rehabilitation with a commercial video game improves sustained attention and executive functions in multiple sclerosis: a pilot study ItalyRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=35, NFinal=34 |
Population: Intervention group (n=18): Mean age=44.64yr; Gender: males=4, females=14; Disease course: RRMS=18; Mean EDSS=3.25; Mean disease duration=13.28yr.
Control group (n=17): Mean age=42.99yr; Gender: males=5, females=12; Disease course: RRMS=17; Mean EDSS=2; Mean disease duration=11.4yr.
Intervention: MS patients with CI were randomized to receive either an 8-wk video game-based cognitive rehabilitation (CR) program at home or to a waitlist control. The CR program used the Dr. Kawashima's Brain Training video game (DKBT; Nintendo, Japan), which has memory, attention, visuospatial processing, and calculations minigames. Assessments were performed at baseline and after treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Stroop Test (SCWT), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).3 |
|
Janssen et al. 2015 The effects of video-game training on broad cognitive transfer in multiple sclerosis: A pilot randomized controlled trial USARCT PEDro=5 NInitial=34, NFinal=28 |
Population: Training group (n=14): Mean age=49.43yr; Gender: males=4, females=10; Disease course: RRMS=14; Mean EDSS=2.86; Mean disease duration=13.00yr.
Control group (n=14): Mean age=44.93yr; Gender: males=3, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=14; Mean EDSS=2.68; Mean disease duration=10.93yr.
Intervention: MS patients were randomized to the training group or a waitlist control group. Participants in the training group underwent an 8-wk hybrid-variable priority training (HVT) program using the Space Fortress video game. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and post intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Rao's Brief Repeatable Battery (BRB) (Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test: 2,3 seconds (PASAT-2, -3); Selective Reminding Task Long-Term Storage (SRT-LTS); Selective Reminding Task Consistent Long-Term Retrieval (SRT-CLTR); 10/36 Spatial Recall Task (SPART); Oral Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Word List Generation Task (WLGT)).1 |
|
Discussion
Video games are discussed together with computer rehabilitation approaches. Please see section 3.2, Discussion.
Table 7. Summary Table of Studies Examining Video GamesExecutive function | Info processing | Memory | |
Improve |
|
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
w | RCT with within-group comparison only |
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (pre-post) |
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that Nintendo's Brain Training video games do improve executive function and information processing speed in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial and one randomized controlled trial with pre-post analysis; DeGiglio et al. 2015, DeGiglio et al. 2016).
There is level 2 evidence that the Space Fortress video game may improve spatial and visuospatial memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial; Janssen et al. 2015).
There is level 2 evidence that the Space Fortress video game may not improve verbal learning and memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial; Janssen et al. 2015).
Nintendo's Braining Training video games may improve executive function and information processing speed, and the Space Fortress video game may improve spatial memory and visuospatial memory in persons with MS.
The Space Fortress video game may not improve verbal learning and memory in persons with MS.
Virtual reality is an immersive intervention in which software simulates a different environment (Munari et al. 2020; Maggio et al. 2020).
Table 8. Studies Examining Virtual Reality for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Munari et al. 2020 Effects of robot-assisted gait training combined with virtual reality on motor and cognitive functions in patients with multiple sclerosis: A pilot, single-blind, randomized controlled trial ItalyRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=17, NFinal=15 |
Population: Intervention group (n=8): Mean age=57yr; Sex: males=3, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=1, SPMS=7; Mean EDSS=5.4; Mean disease duration=17.7yr.
Control group (n=9): Mean age=51.7yr; Sex: males=4, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=2, SPMS=7; Mean EDSS=5; Mean disease duration=13.9yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups received individualized 40min/d session, 2d/wk for 6wks. The robot-assisted gait training + virtual reality (RAGT+VR) intervention group used a G-EO robotics system with three degrees of freedom. The body weight protocol was gradually reduced each week from 30% to 10%. Participants were also exposed to immersive VR while conducting the robotic gait training. Prior to training, patients were encouraged to focus on the scenario at hand. The control group received the gait training without the VR protocol (RAGT). Outcome measures were collected at baseline, after the treatment and at 1-mo f/u. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)1; Phonemic Fluency Test (PFT)2; Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test - Third Edition (RBMT-3); The Novel Task: Immediate and Delayed Recall (NT-IR)2; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale - Revised (WAIS-R): Digit Symbol.2 |
|
Maggio et al. 2020 Do patients with multiple sclerosis benefit from semi-immersive virtual reality? A randomized clinical trial on cognitive and motor outcomes ItalyRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=60, NFinal=60 |
Population: Intervention group (n=30): Mean age=51.9yr; Sex: males=18, females=12; Disease course: RRMS, SPMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified.
Control group (n=30): Mean age=48.2yr; Sex: males=13, females=17; Disease course: RRMS, SPMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Following randomization, all participants completed 3, 60-min sessions/wk of cognitive rehabilitation training for 8wks. The intervention group completed cognitive rehabilitation in a semi-immersive VR system called BTS-Nirvana. The intervention included ecological scenarios that simulate real life. The control group received traditional face-to-face rehabilitation training. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and at the end of the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA); 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART); Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF); Paced Auditory Serial Attention Test (PASAT).3 |
|
Table 9. Summary Table of Studies Examining Virtual Reality
Info processing | Memory | Verbal language skills | |
Improve |
|
|
|
No statistical sig. difference | Munari et al. 2020 (Digit Symbol, PASAT) |
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (pre-post) |
Discussion
Two randomized controlled trials evaluated cognitive outcomes involving a virtual reality intervention. Munari et al. (2020) investigated the effects of robot-assisted gait training with and without virtual reality. All participants received six weeks of biweekly rehabilitation sessions with a robot assisted body weight support device (a G-EO System; Reha Technology, Olten, Switzerland). Those in the virtual reality group experienced a simulation walking in a natural park on a high definition 2D video screen. Maggio et al. (2020) compared a semi-immersive virtual reality cognitive rehabilitation platform (BTS-Nirvana) to in-person cognitive rehabilitation program involving cognitive exercises with pen and pencil. The virtual reality BTS-N system allows participants to perform cognitive exercises while interacting with real-life virtual scenarios with audio-visual stimuli. Both groups received a total of 24 60-minute cognitive rehabilitation sessions over 8 weeks as well as some physical strengthening and gait training exercises.
Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention for both studies, and Munari et al. (2020) include another post intervention follow up at 1 month. The smaller study by Munari et al. (2020) (n=15) did not find statistically significant between-group differences on the cognitive outcomes at any time points. However, they do report a within-group improvement in the VR intervention group on verbal fluency and immediate recall compared to baseline, both after the intervention and sustained at the 1-month follow-up with moderate to large effect sizes. Munari et al. (2020) also report significant improvement in the two-minute walk test in favor of the virtual reality intervention. The larger study by Maggio et al. (2020) (n=60) did find statistically significant between-group differences on general cognition as measured by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment and on the visual spatial memory, spatial memory, and processing speed outcomes. They also report significant within-group pre-post improvements on all the cognitive outcomes, including the mobility outcomes.
In the Maggio et al. (2020) study, participants had mild to moderate cognitive impairment at baseline and by comparison, in the Munari et al. (2020) study, mean baseline Paced Auditory Serial Attention Test scores were less impaired according to visual inspection of the data. In the smaller Munari et al. (2020) study, the mean baseline Mini-Mental State Examination was 28 and the mean EDSS score was 5. Overall, the results from both studies support that virtual reality rehabilitation settings were feasible in patients with mild to moderate cognitive impairment and restricted mobility.
Munari et al. (2020) suggest that a virtual reality platform “provides an enriched opportunity for repetitive practice, feedback information, and motivation for endurance practice, thus promoting cognitive stimulus (visual, auditory, and somato-sensory input) and motor learning” (Munari et al. 2020, p. 158). The choice of the virtual environment may also be relevant since mindfulness and meditation interventions may influence cognitive testing results related to processing speed (See sections 3.14 for Mindfulness and 3.15 for Meditation). The small Munari et al. (2020) study may not have been powered to detect change in cognitive outcomes. However, the 2-minute walk test showed more improvements in the intervention group walking in a virtual park compared to the control group. Exploring the short-term and longer-term impact of different virtual reality settings (i.e., relaxing settings, busy real-life settings) on cognitive testing and cognitive function in simulated life situations would be of interest.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that cognitive rehabilitation in the BTS-Nirvana Virtual Reality environment may improve information processing speed and memory more than traditional cognitive rehabilitation in persons with MS (one randomized control larger trial; Maggio et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that robot-assisted gait training in a virtual reality environment may not improve information processing speed, memory, or verbal language skills more than robot-assisted gait training (one randomized controlled small trial; Munari et al. 2020).
Cognitive rehabilitation carried out in virtual reality may improve information processing and memory in persons with MS.
Robot-assisted gait training in a virtual reality environment may not improve information processing speed, memory, or verbal language skills more than standard robot-assisted gait training.
Shalmoni and Kalron (2020) describe strobic visual training as an intervention that involves intermittently taking away visual input to encourage participants to reduce reliance on online visual training with the goal of improving visual-motor control.
Table 10. Studies Examining Visual Training for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Shalmoni and Kalron 2020 The immediate effect of stroboscopic visual training on information‑processing time in people with multiple sclerosis: an exploratory study IsraelCrossover RCT PEDro=6 NInitial=26, NFinal=26 |
Population: Mean age=47.9yr; Sex: males=10, females=16; Disease course: RRMS=17, PPMS=9; Median EDSS=4.5; Mean disease duration=9.3yr.
Intervention: Initially, participants were randomly allocated to one session of the control or the stroboscopic visual training (SVT) intervention. Each session type was kept as similar as possible including session time and exercise type. Following a 2-wk washout period, participants were crossed over to either the control or SVT intervention. Outcome measures were collected prior to training and immediately after each session. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Mindstream Computerized Cognitive Test (MCCT)1 assessing verbal and non-verbal memory, executive function, visual-spatial processing, verbal function, attention, information processing speed and motor skills. |
|
Table 11. Summary Table of Studies Examining Visual Training
Info processing | Memory | Executive function | Verbal language skills | Global Cog Scores | |
Improve |
|
||||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (pre-post) |
Discussion
One crossover RCT investigated the immediate effect of strobic visual training on cognition, gait, and static balance (Shalmoni and Kalron 2020). Strobic visual glasses were worn while conducting the exercises for the intervention condition and non-strobic glasses were worn for the control condition. The 40- to 50-minute set of exercises for each condition included different drills with a ball: ball catch, wall ball, head turn and catch, and turn and catch. Cognitive performance was evaluated through a computerized software program assessing different cognitive domains (Mindstreams®, NeuroTrax Corp., NY). Cognitive testing occurred pre- and immediately post-training for each condition with a two-week washout in between conditions. Only information processing speed significantly improved immediately post-strobic visual training while non-significant improvements were reported for the visuospatial outcome measures.
These preliminary findings for the effects of strobic visual training on processing speed is encouraging, especially given that processing speed is frequently affected in MS. Shalmoni and Kalron (2020) note their findings agree with previous studies in athletes whereby some perceptual abilities were enhanced with strobic visual training. Further research in PwMS could include the Symbol Digit Modalities Test to assess processing speed since this measure is validated in the MS population.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that strobic visual training may improve information processing speed but not memory, executive function, attention, verbal function, or global cognitive scores (one crossover RCT study; Shalmoni and Kalron, 2020).
Strobic visual training may improve processing speed, but not other cognitive domains in persons with MS.
Neurofeedback through EEG provides participants real-time visual feedback of their brain activity patterns. The neurofeedback approach described by Kober et al. (2019) requires EEG headwear, a portable 10-channel EEG amplifier (NeXus-10 MKII, Mind Media B.V.), and a laptop with the BioTrace + software. The participant can watch on the laptop their own brain activity depicted by vertically moving bars presented on the screen while wearing the EEG headwear. Kober et al. (2019) propose that upregulating the EEG sensorimotor rhythm (SMR, 12–15 Hz) leads to cognitive improvements by way of reducing sensorimotor interferences. They report this approach has been trialed with success in healthy controls, as well as stroke and acquired brain injury research settings (Kober et al. 2019).
Table 12. Studies Examining Neurofeedback for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Kober et al. 2019 Self-regulation of brain activity and its effect on cognitive function in patients with multiple sclerosis – First insights from an interventional study using neurofeedback AustriaPre-post NInitial=14, NFinal=14 |
Population: Mean age=38.9yr; Sex: males=7, females=7; Disease course: RRMS=13, SPMS=1; Median EDSS=2.3; Mean disease duration=9.0yr.
Responder group (n=7): Mean age=36.9yr; Sex: males=3, females=4; Disease course: RRMS=6, SPMS=1; Median EDSS=3.0; Mean disease duration=13.4yr.
Non-responder group (n=7): Mean age=41.0yr; Sex: males=4, females=3; Disease course: RRMS=7; Median EDSS=2.0; Mean disease duration=7.2yr. Intervention: The intervention involved 10 at-home, neurofeedback training sessions in a 3-4-wk period using a tele-rehabilitation system. This system consisted of an EEG headset, EEG amplified, laptop, a server to encrypt data, and a therapist system to monitor data. The first session was a baseline run. During the subsequent sessions, participants received visual feedback of their sensorimotor rhythm, and the aim was to increase the height of the middle bar while keeping the outer two bars constant. If participants were successful in increasing the height of the bar, they were rewarded with points. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and post intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36; 10/36-SPART; SPART); Word List Generation Test (WLGT); Selective Reminding Test (SRT); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)).1 |
|
Table 13. Summary Table of Studies Examining EEG Neurofeedback
Memory | Executive function | |
Improve |
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (pre-post) |
Discussion
One pre-post test study utilizing a neurofeedback approach evaluated objective cognitive outcomes (Kober et al. 2019). The intervention spanned 3-4 weeks and used a tele-rehabilitation system to conduct 10 at-home sessions of neurofeedback with the goal of upregulating the sensory motor rhythm. Participants were provided with the appropriate equipment, including an EEG headset. The goal of the sessions was to increase the height of the sensory motor rhythm power bar on the screen, but not the theta power (4–7 Hz), or beta power (21–35 Hz) power bars. A reward was given if the participant was successful. The biofeedback training was remotely monitored by a therapist available by chat. Responders on the cognitive outcomes were described as those who improved by a critical difference value defined by the smallest difference between the pre and post measurements of a single person, accounting for random fluctuations. Further, a post-hoc binomial statistical analysis concluded that the seven out of fourteen people who met the responder criteria after training was higher than would be predicted by chance alone (p=0.0002). The responder group improved on verbal long term memory, visual-spatial long-term memory, long term memory, executive function outcomes, and overall BRB-N scores. The result of this preliminary study highlights that upregulation of sensory motor rhythm through visual feedback may improve cognitive function in some PwMS, but there are individual differences in response to the training.
Conclusion
There is level 4 evidence that neurofeedback training may improve long-term memory and executive function (one pre-post study; Kober et al., 2019).
Neurofeedback training may improve long-term memory and executive function in persons with MS.
Robotic gait devices may be utilized for gait training in rehabilitation. This section includes a study using a robotic gait device as the independent variable. Gait training without robotic devices is discussed separately in section 3.26.8 of this module. Robotic gait devices may require additional cognitive effort and skills to don and use the device compared to walking without robotic devices. However, robotic-powered gait devices may also help reduce the cognitive and physical effort to walk for people with significant walking impairments.
Table 14. Studies Examining Robotics for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Androwis et al. 2019 Mobility and Cognitive Improvements Resulted from Overground Robotic Exoskeleton Gait-Training in Persons with MS USARCT PEDro=5 NInitial=4, NFinal=4 |
Population: Mean age =50yr; Sex: males=1, females=3; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified.
Intervention: Following randomization, the robotic exoskeleton group underwent 8, 1-hr gait training sessions over 4wks. Therapists could control the angle of the hip and knee joints along with passive sprung ankle joint. Data collected included step count, distance walked, walk time, up-time, and level of resistance. The control group received conventional outpatient gait therapy that involved active overground walking training at the same frequency. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).3 |
|
Table 15. Summary Table of Studies Examining Robotics
Information processing | |
Improve | |
No statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (pre-post) |
Discussion
Androwis et al. (2019) investigated the effects of an exoskeleton overground gait-training program on ambulation and processing speed. Four participants were randomized to gait training either with an exoskeleton robotic device (n=2) or without the device (n=2). Both groups received eight one-hour gait-training sessions over four weeks. The exoskeleton robotic design provided two degrees of freedom at the hip and knee joints and a passive spring ankle joint. The design allowed powered control of the hip and knee joints through the walking motion. The Symbol Digit Modalities Test and walking outcomes were collected at baseline and following the four-week intervention period. There was improvement on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test for the two participants in the intervention group, with one participant improving their score by 131%. The Symbol Digit Modalities Test scores worsened post gait training for the two participants in the control group. This worsening of the Symbol Digit Modalities Test in the control gait-training group is inconsistent with other non-robotic gait-training research; Sandroff et al. (2016) report improvement in the Symbol Digit Modalities Test scores post gait training. In the small RCT by Androwis et al. (2019), data collection was not blinded, and the study involved a very small sample size.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that robotics may not improve any measure of cognition more than gait training in persons with MS (one very small randomized controlled trial; Munari et al. 2020).
Robotic-assisted gait training may not improve cognitive impairment more than gait training alone in persons with MS.
Spaced learning approaches involve temporal dispersion of learning over time. For example, information is reviewed multiple times at different time points rather than “cramming,” in which information is reviewed multiple times in the same sitting.
Retrieval practice involves testing or quizzing oneself on the materials to be learned. Retrieval practice has robust effects in healthy populations for improving memory in comparison to other learning approaches (Karpicke and Roediger 2008).
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Sumowski et al. 2013 Retrieval practice is a robust memory aid for memory-impaired Persons with MS USAPre-Post NInitial=12, NFinal=12 |
Population: Mean age=49.42yr; Gender: males=0, females=12; Disease course: RRMS=10, SPMS=2; Disease severity: Unspecified; Mean disease duration=15.67yr. Intervention: MS patients with severe memory impairment received memory retrieval practice to aid with recall of verbal paired associates under 3 learning conditions: massed restudy (MR), spaced restudy (SR), or retrieval practice (RP). Verbal paired associate (VPA) recall was tested after a short delay (30min) or a long delay (1wk). Cognitive Outcome Measures: Verbal Paired Associates (VPA) recall.1 |
|
Sumowski, Chiaravalloti, and DeLuca 2010 Retrieval practice improves memory in multiple sclerosis: clinical application of the testing effect USAPre-Post NInitial=48, NFinal=48 |
Population: MS population (n=32): Mean age=48.0yr; Gender: males=3, females=29; Disease course: RRMS=21, SPMS=6, PPMS=4, PRMS=1; Disease severity: Unspecified; Mean disease duration=12.8yr.
Healthy controls (n=16): Mean age=47.6yr; Gender: males=1, females=15.
Intervention: MS patients and healthy controls (HC) received memory retrieval practice where subjects studied 48 verbal paired associates (VPA) in 3 different learning conditions: massed restudy (MR), spaced restudy (SR), and spaced testing (ST). In the MR condition, the initial VPA was immediately followed by two restudy trials. For the SR condition, initial VPA presentation was followed by 3 filler trials (other VPAs), a restudy trial, 6 filler trials, and a second restudy trial. The variation in the schedule of presentation was to isolate the effect of spaced learning on memory. For the ST condition, initial VPA presentation was followed by 3 filler trials, a test trial, 6 filler trials, and a second test trial. A subgroup analysis was conducted within MS subjects with and without memory impairment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Verbal Paired Associates (VPA) correct responses.1 |
|
Goverover et al. 2009 A functional application of the spacing effect to improve learning and memory in persons with multiple sclerosis USAPCT NInitial=38, NFinal=38 |
Population: MS participants (n=20): Mean age=48.4yr; Gender: males=4, females=16; Disease course: RRMS=13, PPMS=3, SPMS=4; Disease severity: Unspecified; Mean disease duration=10.6yr.
Healthy controls (n=18): Mean age=41.4yr; Gender: males=6, females=12.
Intervention: Participants' performance on the acquisition of everyday functional tasks was assessed under two conditions: spaced learning trials (trials distributed over time) and massed learning trials (consecutive learning trials). Tasks in the spaced condition were presented to the participants three times with 5-min breaks between each trial. Tasks in the massed condition were presented three consecutive times. Tasks presented included route learning tasks and paragraph learning tasks. Assessments were performed immediately and 30min following the learning task trials. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Number of elements remembered from learning tasks.3 |
|
Table 17. Summary Table of Studies Examining Spaced Learning
Memory | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (pre-post) |
Discussion
Three non-RCT study designs investigated spaced learning approaches in particular against other learning strategies for improving memory. Sumowski et al. (2013; 2010) also compared the effects of retrieval practice with spaced or mass learning in two studies with within-subject designs. The results support that for optimizing memory, retrieval practice is superior to spaced learning, which in turn is superior to mass learning. Similarly, the results of the Goverover et al. (2009) study support the superiority of spaced learning over mass learning for improving memory. The first study by Sumowski et al. (2010) included 32 MS subjects and 16 age-matched healthy controls with comparable baseline IQ according to the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. The second study followed the same within-subject design and experimental protocol, evaluating an independent sample of 12 participants with MS with severe memory impairment at baseline (Sumowski et al. 2013). Severe memory impairment was defined as scoring less than or equal to the second percentile on delayed recall of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, Revised. Retrieval practice improved on the short- and long-term recall of paired word associations for all MS participants in both trials, and with moderate to large effect sizes at the group level.
However, as the authors point out, retrieval practice will only improve memory relevant to everyday life activities if PwMS use the technique. Sumowski et al. (2013) provide the following example:
Patients wishing to learn information in a newspaper article, training manual, or textbook may engage in intermittent self-quizzing throughout their reading (i.e., after each paragraph or page). This act of retrieval practice will result in greater subsequent memory than rereading the information multiple times. (p. 1945)
The simplicity of self-quizzing and its robust effect on memory suggest this approach could be applied in practice for PwMS.
Conclusion
There is level 2 evidence that spaced learning improves memory compared to mass learning (one prospective controlled trial and two pre-post studies; Goverover et al. 2009, Sumowski et al. 2010, and Sumowski et al. 2013).
There is level 2 evidence that retrieval practice improves memory with mild or advanced cognitive impairment to a greater extent than spaced learning or mass learning approaches (two pre-post studies; Sumowski et al. 2010 and Sumowski et al. 2013).
Spaced learning improves memory more than mass learning.
Retrieval practice learning improves memory more than spaced or mass learning in persons with MS with mild or severe cognitive impairment at baseline.
Prospective memory challenges are common in PwMS, and affect daily functioning (i.e., remembering to do an intended task such as take a medication or attend an appointment). Prospective memory impairments (forgetfulness) may negatively affect quality of life. Cue salience is a technique proposed to help with prospective memory. Van Benthem et al. (2015) explains cue salience as follows:
Memory cues have features that render them hypothetically more likely (high-salience) or less likely (low-salience) to act as signals for the prospective memory task. Greater salience or prominence of the cue is associated with better prospective remembering. For example, spatially displacing a letter cue (e.g., a b c) within a string of letters improved prospective memory. Although all letters were located within the field of view, the slight displacement of the letter cue appeared to increase its salience, as compared to conditions where the letter cue was not displaced. (Van Benthem et al. 2015, p.367)
The term prospective memory refers to the ability to remember to carry out a future intended action at the appropriate time (McDaniel and Einstein 2007). This type of memory is clearly important in day-to-day life, from tasks as simple as taking one's medication on time, to as critical as being able pick up one's child from an activity at the right time. Prospective memory also requires intact higher executive function, such as developing strategies to ensure these actions are carried out at the appropriate time and place (McDaniel and Einstein 2007).
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Dagenais et al. 2016 Prospective memory in multiple sclerosis: The impact of cue distinctiveness and executive functioning CanadaPre-Post NInitial=57, NFinal=57 |
Population: MS Participants (n=39): Mean age=45yr; Gender: males=8, females=31; Disease course: RRMS=27, PPMS=5, SPMS=5, clinically isolated syndrome=2; Median EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration=12.0yr.
Healthy Controls (n=18): Mean age=39.61yr; Gender: males=6, females=12.
Intervention: MS participants were assessed on a prospective memory task that varied the cue salience (text bolding of cue words). MS participants were categorized based on their executive functioning (high-, low-executive) as measured by an executive function battery of tests. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Prospective Memory Task (PM)3 |
|
Table 19. Summary Table of Studies Examining Cue Salience
Memory | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (pre-post) |
Discussion
In one pre-post study, the authors aimed to determine whether using cue salience, a technique in which cue words are bolded (salient) while non-important words are not bolded (non-salient), could improve performance on a modified Prospective Memory Task (Dagenais et al. 2016). It is important to note that there was no baseline measurement, meaning there was no measure of prospective memory without cue salience done for comparison after using cue salience task. Thus, there was no true intervention in this study; instead, the authors simply compared whether PwMS respond to cue salience similarly to normal controls. The authors noted that healthy controls performed nearly perfectly on the multiple-choice questions after performing the cue salience task, while MS participants often failed to detect prospective cues. This study does confirm that prospective memory deficits are apparent in PwMS. Interestingly, it was found that PwMS with higher executive function, based on numerous tasks administered as part of a comprehensive cognitive battery, responded to the cue-salience task similarly to controls.
Conclusion
There is level 4 evidence that cue salience may improve prospective memory in both high- and low-executive functioning persons with MS (one pre-post trial; Dagenais et al. 2016).
Preliminary evidence suggests that cue salience may improve prospective memory in persons with MS.
One strategy used to assist with memory is Selective Reminding, a repetition technique (Slamecka and McElree 1983). In such techniques, it is postulated that repetition leads to better recall. In the Selective Reminding Test specifically, the stimulus items are presented, and the participant is asked to immediately recall as many items as they can. Subsequent learning trials are employed, but each time only the items that the participant did not recall on the previous trial are presented, followed by another recall trial. Learning trials persist for either a set number of trials, or until a specific threshold is reached. The benefit of this type of test is that recall, and recognition can also be tested after a delay period (retrospective memory). Selective reminding has similarities with retrieval practice techniques to enhance memory. Retrieval practice has a larger effect than spaced and mass learning techniques for improving memory in PwMS (discussed in section 3.8 of this module).
Table 20. Studies Examining Selective Reminding for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
McKeever et al. 2019 Selective reminding of prospective memory in Multiple Sclerosis USARCT PEDro=6 NInitial=43, NFinal=43 |
Population: MS Selective Reminding group (n=11): Mean age=51.4yr; Sex: males=2, females=9; Disease course: RRMS=9, PPMS=2; Mean EDSS=5.0; Mean disease duration=9.8yr.
MS control one learning trial group (n=10): Mean age=48.9yr; Sex: males=2, females=8; Disease course: RRMS=7, SPMS=2, PRMS=1; Mean EDSS=5.0; Mean disease duration=9.8yr.
Healthy adult Selective Reminding group (n=13): Mean age=41.6yr; Sex: males=5, females=8; Mean EDSS=0.1.
Control Healthy adult one learning trial group (n=9): Mean age=44.9yr; Sex: males=2, females=7; Mean EDSS=0.6. Intervention: Both the MS and the healthy adult groups were randomly allocated to either the selective reminding paradigm or the control one learning trial protocol. Participants completed their assigned intervention and outcome measures were completed at the end of the session. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Selective Reminding Prospective Memory Paradigm (SRPM)1 |
|
Chiaravalloti et al. 2003 Can the repetition effect maximize learning in multiple sclerosis? USPCT NInitial=84, NFinal=84 |
Population: : MS participants (n=64): Mean age=45.6yr; Gender: males=14, females=50; Disease course: RRMS=21, PPMS=18, SPMS=25; Mean EDSS=4.5; Mean disease duration=9.22yr.
Healthy controls (n=20): Mean age=42.3yr; Gender: males=4, females=16.
Intervention: MS participants were given a modified Selective Reminding Test (SRT), a list of 10 words to remember in a selective reminding format. The trials were repeated until the subject recalled all 10 words on two consecutive trials to a maximum of 15 trials. Subjects in each group were split independently into subgroups (low trial; high trial) based on the median number of trials required to reach 100% recall on 2 consecutive trials. Assessments were performed at 30min, 90min, and 1wk, post intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Selective Reminding Test (SRT).3 |
|
Table 21. Summary Table of Studies Examining Selective Reminding
Memory | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
McKeever (2019) evaluated memory performance after either a one-time presentation of a list of words (stimulus) or after applying the Selective Reminding paradigm (the opportunity to practice selective reminding with repeated trials). Both the healthy controls and PwMS performed significantly better (i.e., had a greater recall of presented words) after the Selective Reminding technique. Healthy controls performed better overall than PwMS on the one-time task, but there was no significant difference between the two groups in the Selective Reminding paradigm.
In the Chiaravalloti (2003) study, a similar Selective Reminding task was tested in PwMS; the Selective Reminder Test counts the number of trials to recall all words, and includes delayed recall and recognition tasks. In the healthy control group, the median number of trials to recall all words was four; for PwMS, the median was eight. All PwMS, regardless of the number of trials required to learn all the words, demonstrated a decrease in delayed recall compared to healthy controls. Interestingly, there was a trend in that PwMS who took more trials to recall all words performed worse on the recall measures compared to PwMS who took fewer trials. These findings suggest that more selective reminding trials may not be an efficient strategy for improving recall in PwMS with impaired learning. However, a limitation of this study is that a within subject comparison of the effects of a low versus a high number of selective reminding trials on delayed recall was not studied.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that selective reminding tasks may improve memory in persons with MS compared to single trial encoding conditions (one randomized controlled trial; McKeever et al. 2019).
Selective reminding tasks may improve memory in persons with MS.
A self-generation learning program seeks to improve new learning based on the principal that a person is better able to remember self-generated information over provided information. The individual is trained to self-generate the information to be remembered, whether this be items important to everyday life activities or in a research setting.
Table 22. Studies Examining Self-generation Program for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Goverover et al. 2018 A randomized controlled trial to treat impaired learning and memory in multiple sclerosis: The self-GEN trial USARCT PEDro=9 NInitial=35, NFinal=35 |
Population: Intervention group (n=19): Mean age=50.15yr; Sex: males=30%, females=70%; Disease course RRMS=12, PPMS=5, SPMS=2; Mean PDDS=3.8; Mean disease duration=11.1yr.
Control group (n=16): Mean age=48.5yr; Sex: males=20%, females=80%; Disease course: RRMS=12, PPMS=2, SPMS=2; Mean PDDS=2.9; Mean disease duration=11.4yr.
Intervention: Following randomization, each group completed six individualized sessions over 3wks. The intervention group received the self-generation learning program (self-GEN) including four parts: in part 1, participants were asked to self-generate and remember words related to multiple presented cues; in part 2, participants were asked which list they recalled better and what helped them recall it; in part 3, the previous two parts were repeated with different stimuli; and in part 4, participants were asked to remember new words and asked how self-generation can be used. Lastly, they were instructed to summarize the activities of the session and what they thought was helpful in a journal that was taken up at the beginning of the next session. In the control group, they completed the same tasks but without the self-generated learning or the transfer instructions. Outcome measures were completed at baseline and within a wk of the last treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Contextual Memory Test (CMT)1; Self-regulation Skills Interview (SRSI)1; Memory for Intentions Test (MIST)1; California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II)1; Memory Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ)1; Awareness Questionnaire (AQ)1 |
|
Chiaravalloti et al. 2019 The application of Strategy-based Training to Enhance Memory (STEM) in multiple sclerosis: A pilot RCT USARCT PEDro=6 NInitial=21, NFinal=20 |
Population: : Intervention group (n=9): Mean age=49.67yr; Sex: males=3, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=6, PPMS=1, SPMS=1, PRMS=1; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=13.9yr.
Control group (n=11): Mean age=45.45yr; Sex: males=6, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=7, PPMS=1, SPMS=1; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=16.2yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups received 8, one-on-one sessions over 4wks. The treatment group received the strategy-based training to enhance memory (STEM) intervention. This included discussion of the memory process, self-generation, spaced learning, and retrieval practice techniques and application to daily life. The control group engaged in non-training-oriented tasks including reading a sentence and recalling target words, word association, learning and remembering names, recalling objects, learning recipes, finance management, and keeping a calendar. Both groups had the same amount of contact with the examiner. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 1wk following the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II)1, Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)2 |
|
Goverover, Chiaravalloti, and DeLuca 2014 Task meaningfulness and degree of CI: do they affect self-generated learning in persons with multiple sclerosis? USPost-Test NInitial=70, NFinal=70 |
Population: Mild-MS group (n=35): Mean age=49.2yr; Gender: males=1, females=34; Disease course: RRMS=28, PPMS=3, SPMS=5; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=134.7mo.
Severe-MS group (n=35): Mean age=47.8yr; Gender: males=6, females=29; Disease course: RRMS=26, PPMS=4, SPMS=3; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=169mo. Intervention: Participants learned two tasks (functional everyday tasks and laboratory tasks) under two conditions (provided and self-generated). Assessments of recall were performed immediately, at 30min, and at 1wk after initial presentation. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Generation Effect Task (GE): Number of items recalled from functional and laboratory tasks.3 |
|
Goverover et al. 2011 Examining the benefits of combining two learning strategies on recall of functional information in persons with multiple sclerosis USPost-Test NInitial=38, NFinal=38 |
Population: : MS participants (n=20): Mean age=47.0yr; Gender: males=2, females=18; Disease course: RRMS=15, PPMS=2, SPMS=3; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=10.8yr.
Healthy controls (n=18): Mean age=40.9yr; Gender: males=3, females=15.
Intervention: Participants were tasked to recall names and faces, appointments, and object locations under 3 different encoding conditions: massed rehearsal, spaced learning, and combining self-generated information with spaced learning (spaced-generated). To control for possible order effects, the task and encoding conditions were counterbalanced across participants. Assessments were performed immediately and 30min after initial presentation. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Memory for names and faces; Memory for object location; Memory for appointments.3 |
|
Basso et al. 2008 Self-generated learning in people with multiple sclerosis: an extension of Chiaravalloti and DeLuca 2002 USPre-Post NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: MS-Memory Impaired (n=10): Mean age=43.20yr; Gender: males=2, females=8; Disease course: RRMS=6; Mean EDSS=5.06; Mean disease duration=13.9yr.
MS-Unimpaired (n=10): Mean age=41.50yr; Gender: males=1, females=9; Disease course: RRMS=6; Mean EDSS=3.70; Mean disease duration=6.4yr.
Healthy controls (n=17): Mean age=46.94yr; Gender: males=5, females=12. Intervention: MS participants were divided into groups based on the presence of mild or average memory impairment. Participants were presented with word pairs according to the paired-associate learning task under self-generated and didactic conditions. Assessments were performed immediately and 20min after task completion. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paired Associate Task: Number of words recalled and recognized: immediate and delayed recall.3 |
|
Goverover, Chiaravalloti, and DeLuca 2008 Self-generation to improve learning and memory of functional activities in persons with multiple sclerosis: meal preparation and managing finances USPCT with Pre-Post NInitial=38, NFinal=38 |
Population: : Healthy control group (n=18): Mean age=40.4yr; Gender: males=6, females=12.
Intervention (MS) group (n=20): Mean age=46.1yr; Gender: males=5, females=15; Disease course: RRMS=16, PPMS=2, SPMS=2, PRMS=0; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=12.1yr. Intervention: All participants completed two meal preparation and financial management tasks. One task in each area was presented in the provided condition, in which all instructions were provided to and read by the participants, and the other task was presented in the generated condition, in which participants were asked to generate (fill in the blank) the necessary items needed to perform each step of the task. Participants were assessed immediately following the study, at 30min, and at 1wk following initial presentation. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Generation Effect Task (GE): Recall of task items and step sequences from self-generation protocol1; Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R)2; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)2; Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS), which includes the Trail Making Test (TMT), verbal fluency test, and color-word interference test2; Boston Naming Test (BNT)2; California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT).2 |
|
O'brien et al. 2007 An investigation of the differential effect of self-generation to improve learning and memory in multiple sclerosis and traumatic brain injury USPre-Post NInitial=69, NFinal=69 |
Population: MS participants (n=31): Mean age=45.42yr; Gender: males=5, females=26. No further information provided.
Healthy controls (n=20): Mean age=38.40yr; males=10, females=10.
Intervention: MS participants performed learning and memory tasks under two conditions: self-generated and provided cues. In the generated condition 16 sentences were presented with the last word missing, as indicated by a blank line. In the provided condition an additional set of 16 complete sentences were presented in which the last word was underlined. Outcome measures were collected prior to and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Generation Effect Task (GE): recall of provided and self-generated words; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R): Digit Span, Logical Memory I and II; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT); Oral Trail Making Test-B (TMT-B); Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT).3 |
|
Basso et al. 2006 Self-generated learning in people with multiple sclerosis USPCT with Pre-Post NInitial=117, NFinal=117 |
Population: : Intervention (MS-MOD) group (n=12): Mean age=47.67yr; Gender: males=1, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=4, PPMS/SPMS=3, other=5; Ambulation index=2.91; Mean disease duration: unspecified.
Intervention (MS-MILD) group (n=10): Mean age=50.40yr; Gender: males=1, females=9; Disease course: RRMS=7, PPMS=1, other=2; Ambulation index=3.10; Mean disease duration: Unspecified.
Intervention (MS-UN) group (n=73): Mean age=43.79yr; Gender: males=12, females=61; Disease course: RRMS=38, PPMS/SPMS=12, other=22; Ambulation index=2.47; Mean disease duration: Unspecified.
Healthy control group (n=22): Mean age=42.36yr; Gender: males=5, females=17. Intervention: Based on the California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II) scores, participants were classified to the MS-Unimpaired (MS-UN), MS-Mildly Impaired (MS-MILD), and MS-Moderately Impaired (MS-MOD) groups. Participants were assigned to conditions in which either the self-generation or didactic encoding procedure was administered first. After completing both conditions, immediate free recall was measured. Twenty minutes later, delayed recall and the recognition test was administered. Subsequently, the name and face learning task, the appointment learning task, the object location learning task, and lastly a recognition test of memory for object location were assessed immediately following the test and after a delay of 20min which was allocated between every test. During the delay period, neuropsychological tests unrelated to the study were administered. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Paired Associate Task: free recall and recognition of paired associates, names, object location and appointments.3 |
|
Chiaravalloti and DeLuca 2002 Self-generation as a means of maximizing learning in multiple sclerosis: An application of the generation effect USPCT NInitial=48, NFinal=48 |
Population: MS participants (n=31): Mean age=45.42yr; Gender: males=5, females=26; Disease course: unspecified; Ambulation index=2.07; Mean disease duration=127.8mo.
Healthy controls (n=17): Mean age=41.2yr; Gender: males=6, females=11.
Intervention: MS participants received a Generation effect protocol consisting of two types of sentence stimuli intermingled. Participants were asked to 'fill in the blank' for a missing word in a sentence, indicated by an underlined space. In some cases, the subjects filled in the space with the 'first word that comes to mind.' In the other cases the space was already filled in with a word. Later, the subjects were assessed based on the underlined words to determine if words generated by the subject were recalled at a higher rate than words provided to the subject by the examiner. Assessments were performed immediately, at 30min, and at 1wk after presentation. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Generation Effect Task (GE): recall and recognition of generated or provided stimuli 1; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV): Digit Span1; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT) 1; Oral Trail Making Test A, B (TMT-A, TMT-B) 1; Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) 1; Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) 1; Boston Naming Test (BNT); Animal Fluency (AF)1; Wide Range Achievement Test-3 (WRAT-3) reading subtest; Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R), local memory I and II.1 |
|
Table 23. Summary Table of Studies Examining Self-generation Program
Improve | |||
Memory | Verbal Learning and Memory |
|
|
Working Memory |
|
||
Visuospatial Memory |
|
||
Visual Memory |
|
||
Recall |
|
||
Executive Function |
|
Bold | PEDro >= 6 |
Regular | PEDro < 6 |
Italic | Non-RCT |
T | Trend to improve |
Discussion
Two higher quality RCTs (Y. Goverover et al. 2018; N. D. Chiaravalloti et al. 2019) and seven lower quality studies include self-generation learning techniques with the aim of improving learning and memory. Traditional neuropsychological memory outcomes did not reach statistical significance for a between-group difference in either of the high-quality RCTs. However, Goverover et al. (2018) include the Contextual Memory Test as the primary outcome, and Goverover et al. (2014) and others include the Generation Effect Task Recall Test in earlier studies. The results of these outcomes more directly evaluating success with the self-generation training are positive. The Contextual Memory Test evaluates immediate and delayed object recall in addition to awareness and utilization of memory strategies. The Generation Effect Task Recall test in effect evaluates the success of using the self-generation technique. After the participant self-generates items to be learned, recall of those items is evaluated immediately and in a delayed fashion. In comparison, recall is not as robust for rote items provided to participants when participants either do not receive training in self-generation or do not apply the strategy.
Encouragingly, Goverover, Chiaravalloti, and DeLuca (2014) also report that participants with severe disability are able to apply the self-generation strategy to improve learning, although their recall remained more impaired than those with less severe MS. The self-generation techniques were taught in the research settings within several or even fewer sessions by leading experts in the field. It is unclear if the same success would be realized in real-world settings by self-taught programs or if led by other health care professionals. Applying the technique also requires the participant to have time to take part in the self-generation. This may help to explain why time-sensitive or structured neuropsychological test scores did not improve, since participants may not have had opportunity to apply the self-generation strategies during the testing. This hypothesis would make the case that clinicians and PwMS should advocate for sufficient time to apply memory strategies in situations that involve new learning or testing.
In the second high-quality RCT by Chiaravalloti et al. (2019), participants in the intervention group received training in three memory strategies: self-generation, spaced learning, and retrieval practice (referred to as Strategy-based Training to Enhance Memory—STEM). The control group took part in non-training-orientated cognitive tasks. While the neuropsychological outcomes did not reach statistical significance for between-group differences in this smaller RCT, medium to large effect sizes occurred on the California Verbal Learning Test in the STEM group. These results support allowing PwMS opportunity to learn and apply memory strategies.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that Self-generation Technique improves contextual recall on tasks where the technique is applied compared to not applying the technique (one randomized controlled trial, one prospective controlled trial, and six pre-post studies; Goverover et al. 2018; 2014; 2013; 2011; and 2008; Basso et al. 2008 and 2006; O'Brien et al. 2007; Chiaravalloti & Deluca 2002).
There is level 1a evidence that teaching the Self-Generation Technique may not significantly improve verbal memory (two randomized controlled trials; Goverover et al. 2018 and Chiaravalloti et al. 2019).
There is level 1b evidence that teaching the Self-Generation Technique may not significantly improve visuospatial memory (one randomized controlled trial; Chiaravalloti et. al. 2019).
Teaching the Self-Generation Technique may improve recall on memory tasks where the technique is applied.
Memory—specifically, learning or immediate recall—is one of the most commonly noted impairments in PwMS (Thornton and Raz 1997). The studies in this section focus on the Story Memory technique to improve the acquisition of new information (learning). The Story Memory technique, also used for traumatic brain injury, is based on the theory that improving the quality of the memory acquisition is the best way to strengthen it. Story Memory combines two specific approaches that improve the quality of acquisition: context and imagery. A mental visual imagery technique is also reviewed separately in section 3.13 of this module.
Table 24. Studies Examining Story Memory for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Krch et al. 2019 Efficacy of the Spanish modified Story Memory Technique in Mexicans with multiple sclerosis: A pilot randomized controlled trial USARCT PEDro=9 NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: Intervention group (n=10): Mean age=33.8yr; Sex: unspecified; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=5.8yr.
Control group (n=10): Mean age =39.5yr; Sex: unspecified; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=5.4yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups received 10 sessions over 5wks. The modified Story Memory Technique (mSMT) includes training on imagery and context. Sessions 1-4 presented stories for which participants needed to complete a visual imagery memory aid. Sessions 5-8 included word lists imbedded in a story, and then participants were asked to visualize the story. Sessions 9-10 focused on application of skills to real-world settings (e.g., shopping). The control group engaged in non-training-specific tasks, including reading the same stories and answering questions, but did not learn to apply imagery and the context of the material. Outcome measures were completed at baseline and within 1wk of the treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R)1; Memory Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ).2 |
|
Boukrina et al. 2019 Brain activation patterns associated with paragraph learning in persons with multiple sclerosis: The MEMREHAB trial USARCT PEDro=8 NInitial=16, NFinal=16 |
Population: : Intervention group (n=6): Mean age=49.33yr; Sex: males=1, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=4, PRMS=1, Unknown=1; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=15.7yr.
Control group (n=10): Mean age=46.2yr; Sex: males=4, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=7, PPMS=2, SPMS=1; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=13.0yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups completed 10 treatment sessions. The intervention group received the modified Story Memory Technique (mSMT), which involved participants reading a story and actively recollecting as much of the story as possible followed by a series of questions on the story. They were also taught how to utilize context and imagery to encourage new learning. The control group received the same stimuli as the intervention group without the context and imagery components. During the first fMRI portion of the study, participants completed a paragraph task during sessions 1-4. This was followed by a word recognition task that included 8 words from the previous phase. Participants were presented with words for 2 seconds and instructed to indicate if the words were presented in the past. Outcome measures and fMRI were completed within 13d prior to the study and within 2wks following the treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Memory Assessment Scale (MAS); Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (RBMT).3 |
|
Chiaravalloti, Moore, and DeLuca 2020 The efficacy of the modified Story Memory Technique in progressive MS USARCT PEDro=8 NInitial=28, NFinal=24 |
Population: Intervention group (n=15): Mean age=55.2yr; Sex: males=4, females=11; Disease course: PPMS=3, SPMS=10, PRMS=1; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=17yr.
Control group (n=13): Mean age=53.3yr; Sex: males=6, females=7; Disease course: PPMS=7, SPMS=6; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=16yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups received 10 sessions over 5wks. The modified Story Memory Technique (mSMT) includes training on imagery and context. Sessions 1-4 presented stories for which participants needed to complete a visual imagery memory aid. Sessions 5-8 included word lists imbedded in a story, and then participants were asked to visualize the story. Sessions 9-10 focused on application of skills to real-world settings (e.g. shopping). The control group engaged in non-training specific tasks, including reading the same stories and answering questions, but did not learn to apply imagery and the context of the material. Outcome measures were gathered at baseline, within 1wk of completing treatment, and 3mos later. Cognitive Outcome Measures: California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II)1; Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (RBMT)1; Awareness Questionnaire (AQ)2; Memory Assessment Scale (MAS), Prose Memory2; Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–Revised (BVMT-R)2. |
|
Chiaravalloti et al. 2013 An RCT to treat learning impairment in multiple sclerosis: The MEMREHAB trial USRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=88, NFinal=78 |
Population: : Treatment Group (n=45): Mean age=48.13yr; Gender: males=11, females=34; Disease course: RRMS=33, PPMS=1, SPMS=6 PRMS=1, Unknown=4; Ambulation Index Score=2.68; Mean disease duration=170.87mo.
Control Group (n=41): Mean age=49.32yr; Gender: males=10, females=31; Disease course: RRMS=22, PPMS=4, SPMS=11, PRMS=0, Unknown=4; Ambulation Index Score=3.13; Mean disease duration=173.37mo. Intervention: The treatment group received 10 sessions of the modified Story Memory Treatment (mSMT) training lasting 45-60min, 2x/wk for 5wks. mSMT intervention focuses on teaching imagery and context skills. In addition, the treatment group received monthly booster sessions after immediate follow-up after intervention, where they either received training to apply mSMT to real-world situations or placebo booster sessions where the participants read a story and answered questions. The control group met with the therapist with the same frequency as the treatment group, focusing on non-training-specific tasks. The main difference was that the control group wasn't exposed to imagery and context training. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, within 1wk following the intervention and 6mos following the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: California Learning Verbal Test II (CLVT-II)1; Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (RBMT)1; Subjective report of functioning; Functional Assessment of MS (FAMS); Frontal Systems Behaviour Scale (FrSBe); Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence - Digit Span, Letter Number Sequencing; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS).2 |
|
Chiaravalloti et al. 2012 Increased cerebral activation after behavioral treatment for memory deficits in MS USRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=16, NFinal=16 |
Population: Treatment group (n=8): Mean age=49.25yr; Gender: males=1, females=7; Disease course: RRMS=5; Mean Ambulation Index=2.13; Mean disease duration=186.71mo.
Control group (n=8): Mean age=46.75yr; Gender: males=1, females=7; Disease course: RRMS=6; Mean Ambulation Index=3.75; Mean disease duration=177.14mo. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to receive modified Story Memory Technique (mSMT) or control procedure in ten 45–60 min sessions, 2x/wk, for 5wks. Assessments were performed at baseline and follow-up. The treatment group was taught context and imagery to facilitate learning, and to improve learning and memory abilities. The control group met with the therapist and engaged in non-training specific tasks. Cognitive Outcome Measures: California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) short-delay free recall.3 |
|
Chiaravalloti et al. 2005 Treating learning impairments improves memory performance in multiple sclerosis: a randomized clinical trial USRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=29, NFinal=28 |
Population: : Disease course: RRMS=17, PPMS=4, SPMS=7; Mean Ambulation Index=2.86; Mean disease duration=135.72mo.
Intervention group (n=14): Mean age=45.14yr; Gender: males=5, females=9; Mean Ambulation Index = 3.21; Mean disease duration=168.07mo. Control group (n=14): Mean age=46yr; Gender: males=6, females=8; Mean Ambulation Index = 2.43; Mean disease duration=100.21mo. Intervention: MS patients with learning disabilities were randomly assigned to undergo either 8 sessions of Story Memory Technique (SMT) or 8 sessions of memory exercises. Patients were also stratified according to degree of learning deficits (mild, moderate, and severe). Assessments were conducted at baseline, immediately following treatment and 5wks post treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R): Digit span; Oral Trail Making Test A and B (TMT-A, TMT-B); Animal Fluency (AF); Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT); WAIS-R: Vocabulary subtest; WAIS-R: block design subtest; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) letter-number sequencing; Symbol Digit Modalities Test – oral version (SDMT); Memory Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ).3 |
|
Dobryakova et al. 2014 A pilot study examining functional brain activity 6 months after memory retraining in MS: the MEMREHAB trial USRCT PEDro=2 NInitial=8, NFinal=6 |
Population: Total study sample (n=8): Gender: males=3, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=7, PPMS=1; Severity: Unspecified.
Treatment group (n=4): Mean Age=40yr; Mean disease duration: 137.5mo. No further information provided.
Control group (n=4): Mean Age=46yr; Mean disease duration: 84mo. No further information provided. Intervention: MS patients with memory impairment were randomized to receive the modified Story Memory Technique (mSMT) rehabilitation protocol or control condition. Patients in the treatment group completed 10 sessions of mSMT, while the control group performed memory exercises at the same frequency. Assessments were performed at baseline, immediately after treatment and at 6mo follow-up. Cognitive Outcome Measures: California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT): Short delay free recall (SDFR).3 |
|
Table 25. Summary Table of Studies Examining Story Memory
Verbal learning and Memory | Full Scale Memory | |
Improve |
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
In a pilot study, Chiaravalloti et al. (2005) aimed to determine the effectiveness of a modified story memory technique, involving imagery and context, with respect to new learning in PwMS. A control group underwent an active control, in that they met with the same therapist as did the experimental group, controlling for professional contact. This initial small pilot study did not find a significant difference between the intervention and control group on measures of information processing speed, mood symptoms, or verbal fluency. However, there was a trend towards improvement on a measure of episodic memory (HVLT-R). Subsequent studies by this same group found the modified Story Memory Technique to be beneficial in a larger, heterogeneous sample of PwMS on measures of verbal learning and memory (Chiaravalloti et al. 2012). These benefits were sustained after one year of follow up (Dobryakova et al. 2014). Further, Chiaravalloti, Moore, and DeLuca (2020) report similar positive results when examining modified Story Memory Technique in a progressive MS cohort. A Spanish version of the modified Story Memory Technique, in a Mexican population, demonstrated similar results (Krch et al. 2019).
Conclusion
There is level 1a evidence that the modified Story Memory Technique does improve verbal learning and memory but may not improve other forms of memory (five randomized controlled trials; Chiaravalloti et al. 2020, Chiaravalloti et al. 2013, Chiaravalloti et al. 2012, Dobryakova et al. 2014, Krch et al. 2019).
The Modified Story Memory Technique does improve verbal learning and memory but does not improve other forms of memory in persons with MS
Ernst et al. (2018) described a mental visual imagery program designed specifically with stepwise visualisation exercises of increasing difficulty to improve autobiographical memory relevant to everyday life. A description of autobiographical memory is cited by Tulving et al. (2002) as the “ability mentally to re-experience personal detailed events within a specific spatio-temporal context” (cited in Ernst et al. 2018, p. 1111). The mental visual imagery program included the following:
The external visualisation of 10 verbal items to imagine and describe in as much detail as possible (e.g., shape, colour, size, etc.), with the complementary visualisation of an action made with the item (e.g., visualise a ladybird and visualise it flying away). The construction phase consisted in figuring out complex scenes, bringing into play several characters and various scenarios. (Ernst et al. 2018, p. 1115).
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Ernst et al. 2016 Functional and structural cerebral changes in key brain regions after a facilitation programme for episodic future thought in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis patients FranceRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=17, NFinal=17 |
Population: Intervention group (n=10): Mean age=38.40yr; Gender: males=4, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=10; Mean EDSS=2.45; Mean disease duration=11.10yr.
Control group (n=7): Mean age=34.71yr; Gender: males=1, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=7; Mean EDSS=1.85; Mean disease duration=8.85yr. Intervention: RRMS patients were randomized to receive either a mental visual imagery (MVI)-based facilitation programme (Intervention group) or a verbal control programme (Control group). Each programme was comprised of six 2-hr sessions, 1 or 2x/wk. Assessments were performed at baseline and after treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Autobiographical Interview for Episodic Future Thought (AI-EFT): number of internal details.3 |
|
Ernst et al. 2015 Using mental visual imagery to improve autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis patients: A randomised-controlled trial study FranceRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=40, NFinal=37 |
Population: : Intervention (n=17): Mean age=42yr; Gender: males=4, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=17; Mean EDSS=2.68; Mean disease duration=10.97yr.
Verbal control (n=10): Mean age=37.4yr; Gender: males=1, females=9; Disease course: RRMS=10; Mean EDSS=2.45; Mean disease duration=10.60yr.
Stability control (n=13): Mean age=40yr; Gender: males=4, females=9; Disease course: RRMS=13; Mean EDSS=2.77; Mean disease duration=11.85yr. Intervention: RRMS patients were randomized to three groups: the mental visual imagery (MVI)-based facilitation programme, the verbal control group (sham verbal programme) or the stability group who received no intervention. The MVI programme consisted of mental visualization exercises over 6 2-hr sessions, 1 or 2x/wk. The verbal control program consisted of a narrative-oriented control programme with the same frequency and number of sessions. Assessments were conducted at baseline and 6mos after treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Autobiographical Interview (AI); Autobiographical Memory (AM); Episodic Future Thinking (EFT) components: number of internal details recalled, number of external details recalled, mean total rating.3 |
|
Ernst et al. 2018 Benefits from an autobiographical memory facilitation programme in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis patients: a clinical and neuroimaging study FranceRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: Intervention group (n=10): Mean age=38.40yr; Gender: males=4, females=6; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.45; Mean disease duration=11.10yr.
Control group (n=10): Mean age=37.4yr; Gender: males=1, females=9; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.45; Mean disease duration=10.60yr. Intervention: RRMS patients were randomized to receive either a Mental Visual Imagery (MVI)-based facilitation program (Intervention Group) or a verbal control program (Control Group). Each programme was comprised of six 2-hr sessions, 1 or 2x/wk. Cognitive outcomes and fMRI assessments were performed at baseline and after treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Autobiographical Interview (AI).3 |
|
Ernst et al. 2013 Autobiographical memory in multiple sclerosis patients: assessment and cognitive facilitation FrancePCT with Pre-Post NInitial=60, NFinal=60 |
Population: : Intervention (MS) group (n=25): Mean age=42.96yr; Gender: males=4, females=21; Disease course: RRMS=100%; Mean EDSS=1.77; Mean disease duration=8.85yr.
Healthy control (HC) group (n=35): Mean age=42.17yr; Gender: males=6, females=29. Intervention: The MS participants were assessed for cognitive functioning and impairment in autobiographical memory (AbM). Of the 25 participants, 10 were included in a mental visual imagery (MVI) subgroup that was constructed to alleviate AbM retrieval difficulties. The control group was also assessed on AbM performance, and 10 of the 35 participants were included in the facilitation group which also underwent assessment twice within a month interval between the two sessions. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Cue-word Modified Crovitz test (MCT); Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI).3 |
|
Ernst et al. 2012 Induced brain plasticity after a facilitation programme for autobiographical memory in multiple sclerosis: a preliminary study FrancePre-Post NInitial=23, NFinal=23 |
Population: MS experimental group (n=4): Mean age=37.25yr; Gender: males=1, females=3; Disease course: RRMS=4; Median EDSS=1.5; Mean disease duration=15yr.
MS control group (n=4): Mean age=39.75yr; Gender: males=2, females=2; Disease course: RRMS=4; Median EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration=13.5yr.
Healthy controls (n=15): Mean age: unspecified; Gender: unspecified. Intervention: RRMS patients received the Autobiographical Memory Facilitation Programme cognitive-based training using mental visual imagery (MVI). The programme consisted of 2-hr sessions/wk for at least 6wks. Assessments were performed at baseline and 2mos later. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Autobiographical Interview (AI) memory assessment: free recall (mean number of internal details, mean total rating), specific probes (mean number of internal details, mean total rating).3 |
|
Table 27. Summary Table of Studies Examining Imagery
Memory | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Three RCTs and two pre-post studies, all by the same lead author, investigated a mental imagery learning program for improving memory. The chosen outcome in these studies is the Autobiographical interview memory assessment for which higher scores where consistently achieved in the intervention groups. To score the Autobiographical Interview, the researcher conducts an interview with the participant to assess recall by counting the number of internal details recalled without probing, and with specific probes provided. The Autobiographical Interview is conducted with no time constraints on the individual. Ernst et al. do not include other cognitive outcomes; therefore, it is unclear how the imagery technique might transfer to objective memory function on other testing. However, on MRI they report a significant and large correlation between improvement on the Autobiographical Interview and increased grey matter volume in the left parahippocampal gyrus only observed in the intervention group (r=0.968) (Ernst et al. 2018), and on fMRI they report significant changes in functional connectivity (Ernst et al. 2016).
A person may engage in mental visual imagery even if not overtly instructed to do so when practising other strategies aimed to enhance cognitive functioning. Variations of different mental imaging strategies were also explicitly included in combination with other memory strategies, and objective memory outcomes improved in participants with MS (see section 3.1; (Kardiasmenos et al. 2008; Rodgers et al. 1996). In the case of the intervention described by Ernst et al., the training involved a minimum of six sessions lasting two hours led by experts in the field, specifically targeting mental visual imagery training. It remains unclear which strategies can be most efficiently and feasibly applied in PwMS to improve objective cognitive function relevant to everyday life. Ernst et al. propose that the Autobiographical Interview provides an objective assessment of memory function in everyday life, and scores improve after mental imagery training.
Conclusion
There is level 1a evidence that mental visual imagery training improves memory on an autobiographical memory interview assessment compared to sham verbal training or no intervention in relapsing-remitting MS. Other objective memory and cognitive outcomes are not reported (three randomized controlled trials and two pre-post studies; Ernst et al. 2018, Ernst et al. 2016, Ernst et al. 2015, Ernst et al. 2013, and Ernst et al. 2012).
Mental visual imagery training may improve memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS on an autobiographical memory interview assessment; other objective memory and cognitive outcomes are not reported.
Mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) derive from ancient meditation techniques, secularized and manualized for use in healthcare (Kabat-Zinn 1990). The index MBI is mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR), originally designed to help people with long-term conditions cope with chronic pain and stress (Kabat-Zinn 1982). A derivative, mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) was specifically developed as a preventative treatment for recurrent depression (Segal, Williams, and Teasdale 2002). Both MBSR and MBCT have high-quality meta-analytic evidence for effectiveness, mainly in the treatment of anxiety and recurrent depression (Fjorback et al. 2011) and also somatization (Lakhan and Schofield 2013). In non-MS populations, meta-analyses support improvements in working memory, autobiographical memory, cognitive flexibility, and meta-awareness (Lao, Kissane, and Meadows 2016). How MBIs work is incompletely understood; in systematic reviews and meta-analyses, MBI practice in general populations is associated with complex patterns of functional and structural plasticity (Young et al. 2018), improvements in stress biomarkers (Pascoe et al. 2017), immune profile, and cellular aging (Black and Slavich 2016). Mediators of improvements in anxiety and depression appear to derive from enhanced mindfulness, cognitive and emotional regulation (Gu et al. 2015), and greater amount of home practice (Parsons et al. 2017). In PwMS, meta-analyses of RCTs confirm MBI effectiveness in treating symptoms of stress, anxiety, depression (Robert Simpson et al. 2019), and fatigue (Robert Simpson et al. 2020), all of which are common factors known to confound assessment of cognition in this population (Nancy D Chiaravalloti and DeLuca 2008).
Table 28. Studies Examining Mindfulness for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Senders et al. 2019 Impact of mindfulness-based stress reduction for people with multiple sclerosis at 8 weeks and 12 months: A randomized clinical trial USARCT PEDro=8 NInitial=67, NFinal=62 |
Population: Intervention group (n=33): Mean age=53.24yr; Sex: males=5, females=28; Disease course: RRMS=24, PPMS=2, SPMS=7; Mean EDSS=4.48; Mean disease duration=14.61yr.
Control group (n=29): Mean age=52.59yr; Sex: males=9, females=20; Disease course: RRMS=17, PPMS=2, SPMS=8, Unknown=2; Mean/Median EDSS=4.72; Mean disease duration=17.93yr. Intervention: Following randomization, both groups received 8 weekly, 2-hr classes and a 6-hr retreat at wk 6. MBSR group followed Kabat-Zinn protocol and were taught to bring mindfulness into their day through meditation, movement, eating, and interpersonal interactions. Gentle yoga, breath work and body scans were taught to facilitate mindfulness. MBSR participants were encouraged to practice for 45mins daily. The comparator education group classes focused on National MS Society pamphlets covering a variety of topics including medications, supplements, fatigue, pain, etc. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, 4wks, immediately post intervention, and at 4, 8, and 12mos post intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)2 |
|
Manglani et al. 2020 Effects of 4-Week Mindfulness Training Versus Adaptive Cognitive Training on Processing Speed and Working Memory in Multiple Sclerosis USARCT PEDro=6 NInitial=135, NFinal=61 |
Population: : Adaptive Cognitive Training (aCT) (n=20): Mean age=44.8yr; Sex: males=4, females=16; Disease course: unspecified; Mean/Median EDSS=4.40; Mean disease duration=12.3yr.
Mindfulness-Based Training (MBT) (n=20): Mean age=46.5yr; Sex: males=4, females=16; Disease course: unspecified; Mean EDSS=4.63; Mean disease duration=10.1yr.
Waitlist Control (n=21): Mean age=46.0yr; Sex: males=6, females=15; Disease course: unspecified; Mean EDSS=4.02; Mean disease duration=11.3yr.
Intervention: MBT and aCT groups attended weekly 2-hr training sessions for 4wks. The adaptive cognitive training focused on attention, information processing speed, executive function, and working memory. It was supervised and included instruction and group discussions. Training occurred using computer games on the Poist Science BrainHQ website. The MBT group was modeled after the Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction Program. This program aims to develop breath and body awareness and meditation practices that develop sustained attention. This was supplemented by 40-min sessions at home practiced for the other 6d of the week. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36; 10/36-SPART; SPART), Word List Generation Test (WLGT); Selective Reminding Test (SRT); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)1; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT))1 |
|
De la Torre et al. 2020 Neurocognitive and emotional status after one year of mindfulness-based intervention in patients with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis SpainRCT PEDro=4 NInitial=60, NFinal=60 |
Population: Intervention (n=30): Mean age=44.3yr; Sex: males=8, females=22; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified.
Control (n=30): Mean age=48.8yr; Sex: males=12, females=18; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: The intervention group received 8wks of Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy for Depression, a modified MBSR program. MBSR consisted of weekly in-person sessions and daily at-home practice. Control group did not receive the MBSR but continued pharmacological treatment. Both groups completed outcome measures prior to commencement of the study and 1yr later. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Wechsler Memory Scale-III (WMS-III) including 6 subtests: Wechsler Attention (WATT), Wechsler Long Term Memory (WLT), Wechsler Short Term Memory (WST), Wechsler Recognition (WREC), Wechsler Learning (WLEARN), Wechsler Animals (ANIM); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3 (PASAT-3)3 |
|
Blankespoor et al. 2017 The Effectiveness of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction on Psychological Distress and Cognitive Functioning in Patients with Multiple Sclerosis: A Pilot Study NetherlandsPre-Post NInitial=31, NFinal=16 |
Population: : Mean age=55.19yr; Sex: males=4, females=12; Disease course: RRMS=5, PPMS=4, SPMS=7; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Group mindfulness training program lasting 8wks with 2.5-hr sessions 1x/wk consisting of meditation practice, didactic teaching, and experience sharing. This also included a 6-hr silence day and take-home exercises. Outcome measures were collected 1wk prior to intervention and 2wks following the end of the intervention. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Multifactorial Meta Memory Questionnaire (MMQ)1; Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)1; Location Learning Task (LLT)1; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)1; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-lll (WAIS-III): Digit Span, Letter-number sequencing test.1 |
|
Table 29. Summary Table of Studies Examining Mindfulness
Attention | Executive Function | Info Processing speed | Memory | Verbal skills | |
Improve |
|
|
|
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
w | RCT reporting within group (pre-post) results |
Discussion
Four studies of variable quality met the inclusion criteria for mindfulness interventions. Objective cognitive improvements were found in the cognitive domains of attention (De la Torre et al. 2020) and visual information processing (Manglani et al. 2020). In non- MS populations, adherence with mindfulness treatment is less than optimal (~60%) (Robert Simpson et al. 2019), which may also be a limitation of the research in MS populations. Manglani et. al (2020) reported positive results on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test after only four weeks of a mindfulness-based intervention; however, a significant portion of the sample was not included in the final analysis. De la Torre et al. (2020) reported improved attention at the one-year follow-up in the mindfulness intervention group receiving 8 weeks of treatment. While the follow-up was complete and prolonged in comparison to other studies, a limitation is that the authors do not provide an analysis in comparison to the control group. This study therefore provides only lower quality pre-post results. When considering acceptability, accessibility, and implementation of mindfulness-based interventions, teachers appear to have a key role in helping PwMS make sense of the practices. Similarly, flexible group programming, either face-to-face or live online, with content tailored for enhanced relevance toward common MS symptoms and shortened meditation practices seem best suited to this population. Steps to improve adherence include recommendation by an MS clinician and email reminders. Post-course booster sessions may be necessary to sustain beneficial effects in the long term (Robert Simpson et al. 2021). Studies including only self-reported cognitive outcomes did not meet the inclusion criteria for this module (Hoogerwerf et al. 2017; R. Simpson, Mair, and Mercer 2017). Self-reported improvements in cognitive functioning are importantly positive findings associated with mindfulness-based interventions.
Conclusion
There is level 1a evidence that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may not improve auditory information processing speed (two randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Manglani et al. 2020, Senders et al. 2018, and Blankespoor et al. 2017).
There is level 1b evidence that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may improve visual information processing speed in persons with MS (one random trial; Manglani et al., 2020).
There is conflicting evidence whether mindfulness-based cognitive therapies improve memory in persons with MS (two randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Blankespoor et al. 2017, De la Torre et al. 2020, and Manglani et al. 2020).
There is level 2 evidence that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may improve attention and verbal skills in relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial reporting pre-post results; De la Torre et al. 2020).
There is level 4 evidence that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may not improve executive function (one pre-post study; Blankespoor et al. 2017).
Preliminary evidence supports that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may improve attention and verbal skills in persons with MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether mindfulness-based cognitive therapies improve memory in persons with MS.
Mindfulness-based cognitive therapies do not improve auditory processing speed but may improve visual processing speed in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence suggests that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may not improve executive function in persons with MS.
Meditation is an umbrella term frequently used to describe a wide-ranging set of mental training techniques with varying purposes, which are commonly used to cultivate specific states of consciousness (e.g., equanimity), or compassion (Nash and Newberg 2013). Theorists separate meditation into two broad categories: focused attention and open monitoring. Meditation practices can be internally focused, externally focused, or both. Perhaps unsurprisingly, diverse meditation practices are associated with different neural activation patterns (Tomasino, Chiesa, and Fabbro 2014) and training is associated with functional and structural neuroplasticity (Fox et al. 2016) in key neural networks associated with the regulation of attention and emotion. Physical, psychological, and emotional effects differ too, though common outcome measures feature repeatedly across diverse meditation styles, such as effects on stress, anxiety, and depression (Sedlmeier et al. 2012). Meditation effects on cognitive functioning have also been investigated in PwMS.
Table 30. Studies Examining Meditation for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Bhargav et al. 2016 Immediate effect of two yoga-based relaxation techniques on cognitive functions in patients suffering from relapsing remitting multiple sclerosis: A comparative study GermanyCrossover RCT PEDro=4 NInitial=27, NFinal=18 |
Population: Age range=20-65yr; Sex: males=5, females=13; Disease course: RRMS; Mean/Median EDSS=≤ 6.5; Mean disease duration=18.16yr. Intervention: The intervention group received the cyclic meditation and was compared to the control group receiving supine rest (shavasana). Assessment was performed before and immediately after a 30-min session. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Trail making Test A and B (TMT-A and B)1; Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST); Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT)2; Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R)2 |
|
Anagnostouli et al. 2019 A novel cognitive-behavioral stress management method for multiple sclerosis. A brief report of an observational study GreecePCT NInitial=148, NFinal=128 |
Population: : Intervention group (n=86): Mean age=44.3yr; Sex: males=21, females=65; RRMS=80, unspecified=6; Mean EDSS=2.9; Mean disease duration=11.4yr.
Control group (n=62): Mean age=42.8yr; Sex: males=17, females=45; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.7; Mean disease duration=9.8yr. Intervention: Pythagorean self-awareness intervention (PSAI) was taught by health professionals during 1-hr weekly sessions. Participants diarized 2x/d PSAI practice. The practice included reflection on diet, physical exercise, sleep, interpersonal contacts, and goal setting for the next day. Participants who declined the PSAI intervention comprised the standard care control group. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and at 8-wk follow-up. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment for Multiple Sclerosis (BiCAMS) (Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R); California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II))3 |
|
Table 31. Summary Table of Studies Examining Meditation
Executive Function | Information Processing Speed | Memory | |
Improve |
|
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
W | RCT with within group comparison only |
Discussion
Mind-body medicine (Senders et al. 2012), meditation specifically, is widely used by PwMS (O'Donnell et al. 2020). In cross-sectional surveys (Levin, Hadgkiss, Weiland, Marck, et al. 2014), better quality of life and lower scores for depressive symptoms are reported. Which meditation style is best for PwMS remains unclear (Levin et al. 2014), given the heterogeneity between interventions. Mindfulness-based interventions have the strongest evidence of benefit, and in PwMS can help with stress, anxiety, depression (Simpson et al. 2019), and fatigue (Simpson et al. 2020). In older adults without MS, differences in how meditation programmes are delivered makes interpretation of results challenging, but in general, dose appears to moderate beneficial outcomes, with threshold parameters for frequency and duration reported in meta-analysis (Chan et al. 2019). Also in meta-analysis, among primary care populations, meditation practice is associated with improvements in stress, anxiety, depression, and pain, where the most robust effects are derived from Mindfulness-based interventions (Goyal et al. 2014).
The two studies (Bhargav et al. 2016; Anagnostouli et al. 2019) describing meditation interventions in PwMS and reporting objective cognitive outcomes are both low quality studies. Each study applied different meditation interventions, enrolling participants with relapsing-remitting MS. While both studies report improved processing speed on at least one processing speed outcome, it is unclear how changes in mood or fatigue may have contributed to these findings. The threshold intensity of meditation practice required to observe benefit is unclear. The study by Bhargav et al. (2016) involved an active control group where participants practiced the Shavasana position. The control group also improved pre-post after Shavasana on two of the cognitive outcomes, although greater improvements for other outcomes were observed in the intervention group.
Conclusion
There is level 2 evidence that meditation may not improve executive function in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial; Bhargav et al., 2016).
There is level 2 evidence that meditation may improve information processing speed in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Bhargav et al., 2016, Anagnostouli et al., 2019).
There is conflicting evidence whether meditation may improve memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Bhargav et al., 2016, Anagnostouli et al., 2019).
Preliminary evidence supports that meditation may improve information processing speed in persons with relapsing-remitting MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether meditation improves memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS.
Psychotherapy involves a variety of approaches which focus on understanding and identifying emotions important to psychological health and well-being. Psychotherapy and behavioural techniques are common approaches for the management of mood disorders in the general population as well as PwMS. These approaches have also been studied as potential interventions for CI in PwMS.
Table 32. Studies Examining Psychotherapy for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Abdolghaderi et al. 2019 Comparing the Effect of Positive Psychotherapy and Dialectical Behavior Therapy on Memory and Attention in Multiple Sclerosis Patients IranRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=45, NFinal=40 |
Population: Dialectical Behaviour Therapy (DBT) Group: Mean age=35.17yr.
Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) Group: Mean age=36.58yr.
Control: Mean age=34.75yr. No further information provided. Intervention: Intervention groups completed 8 weekly individual sessions. The DBT group received “acceptance and patient-centered empathy with cognitive behavioural problem-solving and training of social skills” (Abdolghaderi et al. 2019, p. 3). The CBT group focused on accepting weaknesses and modifying thought processes. The control group received no intervention. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the interventions. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS); Stroop Attention Scale (SAS)3 |
|
Bilgi et al. 2015 Evaluation of the effects of group psychotherapy on cognitive function in patients with multiple sclerosis with cognitive dysfunction and depression TurkeyPre-Post NInitial=108, NFinal=108 |
Population: : : MS patients receiving psychotherapy (n=15): Mean age=41.67yr; Gender: males=3, females=12; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=2.90; Mean disease duration=8.60yr. Intervention: MS patients with both depression and CI received six 45-min sessions of group psychotherapy 2x/mo for 3mos. CI was defined as scores lower than the normative 5th percentile on at least two of the BRB tests. Psychotherapy focused on consciousness-raising, coping strategies, empathy for self and others, depression management skills, cognitive exercises to be performed at home, and hopefulness. Assessments were performed at baseline and after 3mos of therapy. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB) (10-36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART 10/36); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Word List Generation Test (WLGT); Selective Reminding Test: total learning, delayed recall (SRT)); Beck Depression Index (BDI)3 |
|
Table 33. Summary Table of Studies Examining Psychotherapy
Attention | Information Processing Speed | Memory | |
Improve |
|
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Psychotherapy comprises a diverse array of interventions designed to alleviate mental distress through dialogue between patients and therapists (American Psychiatric Association, n.d.). Various types of psychotherapy have been developed, including psychoanalysis, psychodynamic therapy, cognitive behavioural therapy, dialectical behaviour therapy, interpersonal therapy, and supportive therapy. Psychotherapy can be delivered on a one-to-one basis, or in a group setting. Across different types of psychotherapy, many elements are shared such as a focus on describing thought content and emotional experience and therapeutic emphasis placed on relaxation. Furthermore, 'common factors' influence treatment outcomes, such as expectancy, goal consensus and collaboration, therapist empathy and authenticity, therapeutic alliance, positive regard, social desirability, and group effects (Wampold 2015). Factors specific to individual therapies also influence treatment outcomes, such as specific 'ingredients,' duration, intensity, and protocol adherence.
In terms of impact on cognitive functioning, psychotherapy is consistently associated with functional and structural plasticity in domains relating to cognitive and emotional re-appraisal (Weingarten and Strauman 2015). Such findings have been observed with dialectical behaviour therapy in people with borderline personality disorder (Schnell and Herpertz 2007). Cognitive behavioural therapy has well-described beneficial effects among PwMS, including improving symptoms of depression (Fiest et al. 2016), stress (Reynard, Sullivan, and Rae-Grant 2014), and fatigue (Phyo et al. 2018). Cognitive behavioural therapy may also have favourable effects on neuro-inflammation in PwMS (Phyo et al. 2018), but this does not necessarily correlate with any beneficial effects on functional outcomes (Burns et al. 2014). All in, any beneficial effects of psychotherapy on cognitive functioning in PwMS may relate more directly to improvements in well-known moderators, such as stress, anxiety, depression, and fatigue. Two studies included psychotherapy intervention one of which was an RCT (Abdolghaddri et al. 2019). The RCT reported improvements in memory in both the cognitive behavioural intervention group and the dialectical behavioural therapy group but not in the control group. These results support that different psychotherapy approaches may be effective at improving memory. The second pre-post study that delivered non-specific psychotherapy reported an improvement in only processing speed despite the inclusion of a battery of cognitive outcome measures which included memory outcomes (Biligi et al. 2015). In the pre-post study, a significant improvement was also observed on the Beck Depression Inventory. Future studies assessing the impact of psychotherapy on cognitive functioning in PwMS will thus need to control for confounding variables such as mood in their analyses.
Conclusion
There is level 2 evidence that eight weeks of cognitive behavioural therapy or dialectical behavioural therapy may improve memory in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Abdolghaddri et al. 2019).
There is level 2 evidence that eight weeks of cognitive behavioural therapy or dialectical behavioural therapy may not improve attention in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Abdolghaddri et al. 2019).
There is level 4 evidence that group psychotherapy may improve auditory information processing speed but not visual information processing speed in persons with MS who have depression and CI (one pre-post trial; Bilgi et al. 2015).
Preliminary evidence supports that psychotherapy may improve memory but not attention in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence suggests that psychotherapy may improve auditory information processing speed but not visual information processing speed in persons with MS.
Social Cognitive Theory is an effective and widely used theory informing behavioural interventions for promoting health behavior (Bandura 2004), including increasing physical activity in people with MS (Robert W Motl, Pekmezi, and Wingo 2018). Social cognitive theory according to Bandura (2004) “posits a multifaceted causal structure in which self-efficacy beliefs operate together with goals, outcome expectations, and perceived environmental impediments and facilitators in the regulation of human motivation, behavior, and well-being” (Bandura 2004, p. 1). Cognitive rehabilitation approaches may include goal-directed recognition of environment facilitators and barriers for enhancing cognitive function (see section 3.1 Cognitive Rehabilitation, Mixed Non-computer Approaches). However, Coote et al. (2017) report on specifically an education program grounded in social cognitive theory as the independent variable.
Table 34. Studies Examining Social Cognitive Theory Education for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Coote et al. 2017 Effect of exercising at minimum recommendations of the multiple sclerosis exercise guideline combined with structured education or attention control education – secondary results of the step it up randomised controlled trial IrelandRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=65, NFinal=54 |
Population: Social Cognitive Theory Intervention group (n=32): Mean age=43.3yr; Sex: males=4, females=29; Disease course: RRMS=27, PPMS=1, Benign=3; Mean EDSS=3.3; Mean disease duration=6.7yr.
Attention Control group (n=33): Mean age=41.9yr; Sex: males=6, females=26; Disease course: RRMS=27, SPMS=1, Benign=1; Mean EDSS=3.3; Mean disease duration=7.0yr. Intervention: Participants in both groups received a 10wk exercise and education program. The exercise component included both aerobic and strength components and intensity was progressively increased to meet the exercise guidelines for PwMS. There were 6 group exercise classes and weekly telephone calls. For the Social Cognitive Theory group, the education covered topics such as self-efficacy, outcome expectations, and goal setting for health behaviour change. The Attention Control group covered topics such as diet, vitamin D, and sleep. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, post-intervention, and at 3- and 6-mo f/u. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)2 |
|
Table 35. Summary Table of Studies Examining Social Cognitive Theory Education
Information Processing Speed | |
Improve | |
no statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
One study by Coote et al. (2017) reported improvement on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test following a social cognitive theory-based educational intervention in ambulatory, previously sedentary PwMS. Coote et al. (2017) compared the effects of two different educational programs in a high-quality RCT in which both groups also received a 10-week aerobic and strength-training program. The intervention group received education grounded in social cognitive theory for behavior change whereby participants learned about goal setting, self-efficacy, outcomes, barriers, and benefits related to exercise (Coote et al. 2017, p. 3). The control group attention education program covered topics not directly related to exercise (i.e., diet, vaccinations, sleep, vitamins). Fatigue, mood, physical function, and cognitive function on the SDMT where assessed post intervention and at the 3- and 6-month follow-up as secondary outcomes. The study did not include other cognitive outcomes. Ideally, improved longer-term adherence with the minimum recommendations for exercise in the intervention group would be associated with larger and more sustained benefits compared to the control group. However, the a priori between-group analyses did not reach statistical significance for the Symbol Digit Modalities Test, or for the other secondary outcomes. Despite this, only the intervention group improved significantly on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test at three and six months compared to baseline. In addition, only the intervention group significantly improved on measures of depression and anxiety compared to baseline.
The encouraging pre-post results within the intervention group support that a social cognitive theory-based educational intervention improves processing speed, either through a direct relationship, or perhaps through indirect effects on mood. These results are clinically relevant and suggest that larger samples may be required to reach statistically significant between group findings on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. This may be especially the case when the Symbol Digit Modalities Test scores are not impaired at baseline, and when there is an active comparator group compared to a wait list or non-active comparator group.
Within-group change scores in both groups also showed significant improvement at three and six months compared to baseline on fatigue, strength, physical activity, goal setting, and exercise planning outcomes. Reported separately, the intervention group experienced greater improvement on the six-minute walk test primary outcome compared to the control group (Hayes et al. 2017). The Coote et al. (2017) study protocol is feasible, consisting of six group-based exercise sessions followed by the educational component. Phone coaching interactions with the physical therapist also occurred four times over the 10 weeks. Social cognitive theory-based educational interventions guided by physiotherapist-supported exercise show promise for realizing possible benefits on processing speed.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that social cognitive education combined with aerobic and strength exercise may not improve information processing more than attention control education combined with aerobic and strength exercise (one randomized controlled trial; Coote et al. 2017).
There is level 2 evidence that social cognitive education combined with aerobic and strength exercise may improve information processing speed (within-group pre-post results from one randomized controlled trial; Coote et al. 2017).
Social Cognitive Education combined with exercise may improve information processing speed, but not more than Attention Control Education combined with exercise.
Music therapy is defined by the Canadian Association of Music Therapists (2020) as “[using] music purposefully within therapeutic relationships to support development, health, and well-being” (Canadian Association of Music Therapy 2020, para.1). Some studies attribute the beneficial effects of music therapy to changes in neural activation and neuroplasticity (Sihvonen et al. 2017; François et al. 2015; Thaut et al. 2014; Altenmüller et al. 2009). A systematic review reported on a wide spectrum of music-based therapies, but only a few have been explored in MS (Vinciguerra, De Stefano, and Federico 2019).
Table 36. Studies Examining Music Therapy for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Impellizzeri et al. 2020 An integrative cognitive rehabilitation using neurologic music therapy in multiple sclerosis: A pilot study ItalyRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=30, NFinal=30 |
Population: Intervention group (n=15): Mean age=51.73yr; Sex: males=9, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=8, PPMS=3, SPMS=4; Mean/Median EDSS=5; Mean disease duration=9yr.
Control group (n=15): Mean age=51.33yr; Sex: males=10, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=7, PPMS=4, SPMS=4; Mean/Median EDSS=4.5; Mean disease duration=10yr. Intervention: Following randomization, each group underwent an 8-wk rehabilitation program with 6 sessions/wk. The control group received conventional cognitive rehab (CCR) for all sessions, while the intervention group received 3 sessions of CCR and 3 sessions of neurologic music therapy (NMT)/wk. NMT techniques included Associative Mood and Memory Training and Music in Psychosocial Training and Counselling. Each session lasted 60mins. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36; 10/36-SPART; SPART); Word List Generation Test (WLGT); Selective Reminding Test (SRT); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT))1 |
|
Table 37. Summary Table of Studies Examining Music Therapy
Memory | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
This small, randomized controlled trial provides preliminary level 1b evidence that neurologic music therapy may be superior to conventional cognitive rehabilitation for people with multiple sclerosis in terms of memory, mood, and quality of life. A power calculation is not provided, and between-group differences were positive on only two of the outcomes whithin the Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests. The intervention componentry in this trial goes beyond the simple addition of music, for example involving discussions guided by a therapist about feelings in response to music. Furthermore, the lack of reporting on effect sizes limits interpretation of the findings and clinical significance is unclear.
Music therapy has considerable evidence for benefit in the realm of mental health disorders (Gold et al. 2009), as well as in other acquired brain injury populations, about which a Cochrane Review in 2017 reported improvements in communication, upper limb function, gait, and quality of life (Magee et al. 2017). How music therapy works is largely unknown, though 'dose' appears to be important (Gold et al. 2009). Qualitative evidence synthesis suggests a combination of group and specific effects are at play, depending on delivery method (Solli, Rolvsjord, and Borg 2013). It should also be noted that music therapy can have adverse effects, such as an unpleasant emotional experience (Moore 2013), and for people with physical disabilities, additional steps may be required to facilitate participation (Frid 2019).
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that neurologic music therapy combined with cognitive rehabilitation may improve memory more than conventional cognitive rehabilitation (one randomized controlled trial; Impellizzeri et al. 2020).
Music therapy may be beneficial for improving memory in persons with MS.
In this section, music provides a mnemonic strategy to help organize and structure information so that it might be more easily recalled (Moore et al. 2008).
Table 38. Studies Examining Music Mnemonics for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Thaut et al. 2014 Music mnemonics aid verbal memory and induce learning - related brain plasticity in multiple sclerosis USARCT PEDro=5 NInitial=54, NFinal=54 |
Population: Total population (n=54): Mean age=50.3yr; Gender: males=16, females=38.
Spoken group: Mean age=53.3yr; Gender: unspecified; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=4.3; Disease duration: Unspecified.
Sung group: Mean age=50.3yr; Gender: unspecified; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=4.9; Disease duration: Unspecified. Intervention: MS participants were randomized into two groups, a spoken or sung presentation of Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test. Assessments were performed without further presentation of the original word list (M1) after subjects heard and free-recalled a distractor list and after a 20-min non-verbal distractor task (M2). Cognitive Outcome Measures: Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)1 |
|
Moore et al. 2008 The effectiveness of music as a mnemonic device on recognition memory for people with multiple sclerosis USARCT PEDro=4 NInitial=38, NFinal=38 |
Population: : Music group (n=20): Mean age=50.25yr; Gender: males=4, females=16; Disease course: CPMS=7, chronic stable MS=11; Mean EDSS=4.88; Disease duration: unspecified.
Spoken group (n=18): Mean age=53.33yr; Gender: males=4, females=14; Disease course: CPMS=4, chronic stable MS=14; Mean EDSS=4.33; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: MS patients were evaluated on a recognition memory task after randomization to two learning conditions: learning through music or speech. Assessments were performed at baseline and after treatment. Cognitive Outcomes/Outcome Measures: : Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)1 |
|
Table 39. Summary Table of Studies Examining Music Mnemonics
Memory | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Two randomized controlled trials investigated the use of music mnemonics on cognitive impairment in persons with MS (Moore et al. 2008; Thaut et al. 2014). Moore et al. (2008) randomized participants to a music-learning condition or a speech-learning control condition. For both conditions, participants were to recall a list of 15 words from Rey's Auditory-Verbal Learning Test after a 20-minute delay. The music group heard the list of words in a song format to the tune of Skip to My Lou, while the control group heard the list in a spoken format. Participants in both conditions learned an intervening distracter list and then participated in a 20-minute distractor task. Finally, recall and recognition of the words from the original list were tested. There were no significant differences between the two groups in recall based on the Rey's Auditory-Verbal Learning Test.
Thaut et al. (2014) applied a very similar protocol to the Moore et al. (2008) study, except their results were positive in favor of music mnemonics. A statistically significant greater percentage of the words were recalled in the sung group in comparison to the control group on the Rey's Auditory-Verbal Learning Test after the 20-minute distractor. Thaut et al. (2014) also report a significant improvement in pair-wise word order learning in the music group at the end of the last learning trial and the two successive memory trials. In comparing these two studies, the Thault et al. (2014) was a larger trial (54 participants versus 38 in Moore et al. 2008 study). Moore et al. (2008) found a positive association between responders to music mnemonics and less cognitive impairment at baseline. They suggest that for this reason, teaching music mnemonic learning strategies earlier in the disease course may be more beneficial. Thault et al. (2014) propose that music mnemonic learning strengthens the encoding of new memories through the recruitment of a stronger oscillatory network synchronization in the prefrontal area, a hypothesis supported by EEG recordings performed during their study.
Conclusion
There is conflicting evidence whether music mnemonics improve memory compared to spoken words in persons with MS (two randomized controlled trials; Moore et al. 2008, Thaut et al. 2014).
There is conflicting evidence whether music mnemonics improves memory in persons with MS.
Occupation-based interventions involve sessions that teach compensation strategies, routines, and techniques that can be weaved into a participant's occupation (Reilly and Hynes 2018).
Table 40. Studies Examining Occupation Based for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Reilly and Hynes 2018 A Cognitive Occupation-Based Programme for People with Multiple Sclerosis: A Study to Test Feasibility and Clinical Outcomes IrelandPre-post NInitial=12, NFinal=12 |
Population: Mean age=55yr; Sex: males=1, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=5, PPMS=3, unknown=4; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=14.25yr. Intervention: The Cognitive Occupation-Based Programme for People with Multiple Sclerosis (COB-MS) program consists of eight sessions over 2wks that includes two individual and six group-based sessions. Information provided included managing demands of employment and daily life through compensatory strategies and routines and learning new techniques. Outcome measures were collected 1wk prior to the intervention, 1wk following the final session, and 8wks following the final session. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Goal Attainment Scale (GAS)1, Occupational Self-Assessment-Daily Living Scales (OSA-DLS), California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II)2; Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)2; Symbol Digit Modality Test (SDMT)2; Trail Making Test (TMT-A)2; Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult (BRIEF-A)2; Everyday Memory Questionnaire-Revised (EMQ-R)2. |
|
Table 41. Summary Table of Studies Examining Occupation Based
Executive Function | Information Processing Speed | |
Improve | ||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT (Pre-post) |
Discussion
One pre-post test study utilizing a cognitive occupation-based program included objective cognitive outcomes (Reilly and Hynes 2018). A convenience sample of PwMS participated in a 9-week intervention with the main goal of helping participants learn compensation strategies that could be incorporated into daily work. The primary outcome measure was the Goal Attainment Scale, a measure utilized in rehabilitation practice and research, allowing participants to create their own meaningful goals for the intervention (Turner-Stokes 2009). Results of the study highlighted that participants were significantly better able to perform ADLs and IADLs as measured by the Goal Attainment Scale. There were also significant improvements in perceived occupational competency and fewer daily life memory difficulties. However, no significant improvements were observed on the objective cognitive outcomes, the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version, Trail Making Test, and Symbol Digit Modalities Test.
Overall, the results of this study highlight that there is value in having participants define their own goals. Goals related to improving cognitive functioning in daily life were achieved with a cognitive occupation-based program, even when objective cognitive testing outcomes did not significantly improve. Learning compensatory strategies focusing on patient identified goals in an occupational context would be feasible in practice settings. Knowing which compensatory strategies are most effective for which situations would be critical to the success of the intervention. Future research may further improve the success of occupation-based programs by understanding influencing factors such as patient self-awareness, clinician experience, and clinician training opportunities.
Conclusion
There is level 4 evidence that a Cognitive Occupation-Based Program may not improve processing speed or executive function; however, ADLs and IADLs and occupational competence may improve by self-report (one pre-post study; Reilly et al., 2018).
Cognitive Occupation-Based Programme for People with Multiple Sclerosis (COB-MS) may not improve processing speed or executive function, but self-reported performance on ADLs, IADLs and occupational competence may improve.
Action observation training aims to address hand motor deficit in PwMS. This intervention typically involves watching a motor task that utilizes the involved limb (Rocca et al. 2019).
Table 42. Studies Examining Action Observation for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Rocca et al. 2019 Functional and structural plasticity following action observation training in multiple sclerosis ItalyRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=87, NFinal=87 |
Population: Intervention group: healthy control-action observation training (HC-AOT) (n=23): Mean age=45.9yr; Sex: males=12, females=11.
Intervention group: MS-AOT (n=20): Mean age =50.4yr; Sex: males=9, females=11; Disease course: unspecified; Median EDSS=6.5; Mean disease duration=18yr.
Control group: healthy control-control (HC-control) (n=23): Mean age=47.0yr; Sex: males=7, females=16.
Control group: MS-Control (n=21): Mean age=51.5yr; Sex: males=6, females=15; Disease course: unspecified; Median EDSS=6.0; Mean disease duration=16yr. Intervention: Following randomization, all groups received daily, 40-min sessions over 2wks of upper limb rehabilitation. The rehab sessions consisted of 10-min right upper limb passive mobilization, watching three videos, and execution of right-hand daily-life activities. The intervention groups viewed videos of daily-life right-hand and arm action while the control groups watched inanimate landscapes. Both MS participants and healthy controls received the same training with the video content being the only difference. MS participants also received daily standard rehabilitation sessions. Outcome and MRI measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention at 2wks. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Additional Test (PASAT)3. |
|
Table 43. Summary Table of Studies Examining Action Observation
Attention | Information Processing Speed | Memory | |
Improve |
|
||
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
One randomized controlled trial investigated 2 weeks of action observation training combined with standard rehabilitation to assess whether this intervention improves hand function in right-handed individuals with MS and healthy controls (Rocca et al. 2019). Exploratory outcomes included advanced MRI analysis techniques and auditory processing speed on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test. Those in the intervention group watched videos of daily life using right-hand and arm actions and those in the control group watched inanimate landscapes. There were statistically significant improvements on the mean PASAT z scores between baseline and 2 weeks in all the groups. Improvements across all groups may relate to practice effects observed on the PASAT. However, in the MS action observation group, the mean PASAT z score improved by approximately 1 standard deviation. In comparison, smaller improvements were observed in the other groups. Between-group statistical analyses and effect size data were not provided, and authors state in the discussion that no specific effect of action observation was observed on the PASAT (Rocca et al. 2019, p. 1484). From this preliminary study, it remains unclear if there is an additional benefit from action observation compared to standard rehabilitation training for improving processing speed. One advantage of this approach is that watching videos would be highly feasible to implement if effective.
Conclusion
There is level 4 evidence that watching daily life hand movements (action observation) may improve auditory processing speed in persons with MS receiving an upper-limb rehabilitation program (pre-post data in one randomized controlled study; Rocca et al., 2019).
There is preliminary evidence that action observation training added to an upper-limb rehabilitation program may improve auditory processing speed in persons with MS.
Persons living with MS may experience a transient worsening of MS symptoms when the body temperature is elevated, a phenomenon first described by Wilhelm Uhthoff (Selhorst and Saul 1995). Exposure to heat, including temperature elevation with exercise, may temporarily worsen visual function and physical function in persons with MS with heat sensitivity. Cooling approaches include wearing a cooling vest, lowering the room temperature, submersion in cool water, etc.
Table 44. Studies Examining Cooling for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Schwid et al. 2003 A randomized controlled study of the acute and chronic effects of cooling therapy for MS USARCT Crossover PEDro=8 NInitial=84, NFinal=84 |
Population: Mean age=48.1yr; Gender: males=32, females=52; Disease course: Stable or relapsing=74, Progressive MS=6; Mean EDSS=3.3; Mean disease duration: Unspecified. Intervention: MS participants were randomized to receive a single session of low-dose or high-dose cooling therapy acutely (single 60-min session). After 1wk, the groups crossed over and received the alternate acute treatment. One week later, patients were randomized a second time to either high-dose or no cooling therapy chronically (1 hr/d for 4wks). After a 1-wk washout period, the patients underwent the alternate condition for another 4wks. Cooling was performed with a liquid cooling garment (LCG). Assessments were performed at baseline and after cooling sessions. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT).3. |
|
Gonzales et al. 2017 Effects of a Training Program Involving Body Cooling on Physical and Cognitive Capacities and Quality of Life in Multiple Sclerosis Patients: A Pilot Study FranceRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=18, NFinal=18 |
Population: Intervention group (n=9): Mean age=50.04yr; Sex: males=3, females=6; PPMS=40%, SPMS=60%; Mean EDSS=5.06; Mean disease duration=9.3yr.
Control group (n=9): Mean age=49.12yr; Sex: males=3, females=6; PPMS=40%, SPMS=60%; Mean EDSS=5.11; Mean disease duration= 10.2yr. Intervention: Following stratified randomization, both groups completed a 7-wk exercise program including Nordic walking, cycle ergometers, range of motion exercises, and exercises with a ball to moderate intensity. During training, the intervention group wore a cooling vest while the control group wore a cotton t-shirt. Outcome measures were collected 1d before and 1d after the training program. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A), Trail Making Test-B (TMT-B); Isaacs Set Test (IST).3. |
|
Geisler et al. 1996 Cooling and Multiple Sclerosis: Cognitive and Sensory Effects USAPre-Post NInitial=16, NFinal=16 |
Population: MS Participants (n=8): Mean age=43yr; Disease course: RRMS=8; No further information provided.
Healthy controls (n=8): Mean age=27.4yr; Gender: unspecified. Intervention: Heat-sensitive MS participants and healthy controls underwent 2hrs of cooling on one day and 2hrs of sham cooling on another day. Cooling involved lowering the core body temperature by one degree or more with the use of a cooling jacket. Assessments were performed in the normal and cooled states. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R) Digit Span; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test: 3-, 2-second (PASAT-3, -2); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Trail Making Test A, B (TMT-A, -B); Stroop Test (SCWT); Boston Naming Test (BNT); Complex Figure Test (CFT): immediate recall, delayed recall; Selective Reminding Test (SRT); Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT).3. |
|
Table 45. Summary Table of Studies Examining Cooling
Executive Function | Information Processing | Memory | Verbal Fluency | |
Improve |
|
|
||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
||
Worsen |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Three studies report conflicting results on cooling interventions and their effect on cognitive symptoms. All three studies apply diferent methods in terms of the timing and degree of cooling in association with the timing of the cognitive testing. In the Gonzales et al. (2017) study, processing speed and verbal fluency improved with cooling, while executive function did not. Baseline cognitive scores in the control and intervention groups are not matched in the Gonzales et al (2017) study; therefore, the results are difficult to interpret. In the study by Geilser et al. (1996), memory testing conducted while the body temperature was one degree below the normal resting body temperature was associated with worsening memory scores in both the healthy control and MS groups. For both healthy controls and people with MS, a normal body temperature may be important for optimal cognitive performance. Clinically, these findings would support that cooling below normal resting temperatures may not improve cognitive performance. In addition, trying to complete challenging cognitive tasks while the body temperature is elevated (i.e., immediately after rigorous exercise) might not be advisable for optimal cognitive performance.
Conclusion
There is conflicting evidence whether cooling may improve information processing in persons with MS (two randomized controlled trials; Gonzales et al. 2017; Schwid et al. 2013).
There is level 1b evidence that a walking, cycling, and ROM exercise program while wearing a cooling garment may improve verbal fluency compared to the same exercise program without a cooling garment in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Gonzales et al. 2017).
There is level 4 evidence that memory is worse when the body temperature is lowered by one degree Celsius compared to a resting control temperature in persons with MS (one pre-post study; Geisler et al. 1996).
An exercise program with a cooling garment may improve verbal fluency in persons with MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether cooling garments improve information processing in persons with MS.
Preliminary evidence suggests that cooling below the resting normal temperature may worsen memory in persons with MS.
Art therapy is a complex intervention that has been studied in other patient populations to help with cognition, mood symptoms, or quality of life.
Table 46. Studies Examining Art for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Van Geel et al. 2020 Effects of a 10-week multimodal dance and art intervention program leading to a public performance in persons with multiple sclerosis - A controlled pilot-trial BelgiumPre-post NInitial=18, NFinal=17 |
Population: Dance group (n=7): Age range=29-52yr; Sex: males=0, females=7; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=3-21yr.
Art group (n=10): Age range=40-65yr; Sex: males=1, females=9; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=6-21yr. Intervention: Participants were allocated to groups based on preference. Both groups had 90-min sessions, 2x/wk over 10wks. The dance group received choreo-based dance therapy. The sessions included a 10-min warm up, 70-min training, and 10-min cooldown. The training included three choreographies of increasing difficulty. The art group included poem recitation, creating paintings, photography, and videography. The main goal for both groups was to work toward presenting their performance and work at an exhibition for an audience after the intervention. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 2wks after the live performance. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Modified Fatigue Impact Scale (MFIS)1; Symbol Digits Modalities Test (SDMT)2; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)2; Dual Task: Word List Generation (WLG) and subtraction2. |
|
Table 47. Summary Table of Studies Examining Art
Visual Information Processing | Auditory Information Processing | Memory | |
Improve |
|
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
This small-scale pilot study provides level 4 evidence that people with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis (RRMS) may benefit from art therapy, with statistically significant improvements noted in information processing speed and dual task performance. Non-randomized study design and a lack of participant blinding (with intervention group allocation based on individual preference) indicates an elevated risk of bias, whilst poor reporting of participant sociodemographic and clinical characteristics limits the interpretation and thus relevance of findings. In non-MS populations, art therapy has been associated with improvements in anxiety (Abbing et al. 2018), mood and cognition (Masika, Yu, and Li 2020), and quality of life (Emblad and Mukaetova-Ladinska 2021). Art therapy is a heterogenous, complex intervention (Masika, Yu, and Li 2020); arguably, standardization is counter to the philosophy of art, which is problematic when seeking to test an art-based intervention under trial conditions. Qualitative systematic review evidence highlights that art therapy appears to be acceptable to many but needs careful application and tailoring to make it accessible in the context of physical illness (Scope, Uttley, and Sutton 2017). Further, adaptations are likely required in the context of physical and cognitive impairment (Luzzatto et al. 2017).
Conclusion
There is level 4 evidence that team-based artistic therapy, consisting of photography, painting, poetry, and videography, may improve visual information processing speed and memory but not auditory information processing speed in relapsing-remitting MS (one pre-post study; Van Geel et al. 2020).
Preliminary evidence suggests that team-based artistic therapy may improve visual information processing speed and memory but may not improve auditory information processing speed in relapsing-remitting MS.
Different diets or dietary behaviors have long been associated with worsening or improved disability outcomes in MS (Murray 2005). However, there is little research exploring cognitive outcomes in relation to diet specifically and systematic evaluation of dietary interventions is challenging. In a large cross-sectional study, a healthy lifestyle composite measure, including a healthier diet and healthy weight, was associated with significantly lower odds of cognitive impairment (0.67; 95% CI 0.55-0.79) (Fitzgerald et al. 2018). In the Fitzgerald et al. 2018 study, patients provided self-report cognitive symptoms through participation in the North American Research Committee on MS (NARCOMS) Registry (https://www.narcoms.org/).
Table 48. Studies Examining Diet for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Lee et al. 2017 A Multimodal, Nonpharmacologic Intervention Improves Mood and Cognitive Function in People with Multiple Sclerosis USAPre-Post NInitial=21, NFinal=19 |
Population: Mean age=51yr; Sex: males=5, females=14; Disease course: PPMS=2, SPMS=17; Mean EDSS=6.2; Mean disease duration=13.6yr. Intervention: For each participant the intervention included a modified paleolithic diet, exercises (stretching exercises, strengthening exercises), neuromuscular electrical stimulation, and stress management (meditation and self-massage). Daily home logs were kept recording the dosage of each aspect of the intervention. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and at 3, 6, 9, and 12mos after the start of the multimodal intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Cognitive Stability Index (CSI); Cognitive Screening Test (CST); Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS); Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III): Matrix Reasoning; WAIS-III: Similarities; Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR)3. |
|
Table 49. Summary Table of Studies Examining Diet
Executive Function | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
This small pre-post feasibility study provides level 4 evidence that a modified paleolithic diet, as part of a multimodal intervention including electrical stimulation, exercise, and stress management, is associated with significant pre-12-month follow-up improvements in memory/learning, attention, language, complex verbal fluency, and verbal and visual reasoning, but not cognitive processing/response speed. Although the authors performed multiple correlation analyses, the complexity of the intervention makes it difficult to pick apart direct effects, and it seems likely that the interventions' components interact to produce observed effects (Gardner, de Bruijn, and Lally 2011). The rationale for a direct impact of diet on cognitive function in PwMS is largely theoretical, though the potential for indirect effects is logical in that nutrition has a fundamental role in the building blocks of neural substrate and functioning (Benau et al. 2021), malnutrition is linked with increased risk for neural decline and dementia (Bianchi, Herrera, and Laura 2021), and the Mediterranean diet has well-described beneficial impact on metabolic markers of inflammation and cardiovascular risk (Papadaki, Nolen-Doerr, and Mantzoros 2020). A useful feasibility finding from this study is that participants reported >94% adherence to the dietary intervention, suggesting the diet is acceptable and sustainable in this context. A limitation with pre-post assessment such as in this study is the potential for observed effects to be unrelated, or only partially related to the intervention components, with the potential for a Hawthorne effect (Sedgwick and Greenwood 2015).
Conclusion
There is level 4 evidence that a modified paleolithic diet combined with electrical stimulation, exercise, and stress management may improve executive functioning (one pre-post study; Lee et al. 2017).
Preliminary evidence suggests that a modified paleolithic diet combined with electrical stimulation, exercise, and stress management may improve executive functioning in persons with MS.
Dual-task training involves simultaneously performing a cognitive task and a motor task together—for example, walking on the treadmill while counting backwards. The difficulty and the duration of the cognitive and the motor tasks performed together may vary. Studies comparing cognitive dual-task motor training to an active motor training control condition are included in this section. Cognitive dual-task balance training as an exercise intervention compared to a sedentary control condition is covered in the exercise section 3.26.6.
Table 50. Studies Examining Cognitive-Motor Dual Task Training for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Jonsdottir et al. 2018 Intensive Multimodal Training to Improve Gait Resistance, Mobility, Balance and Cognitive Function in Persons With Multiple Sclerosis: A Pilot Randomized Controlled Trial ItalyRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=42, NFinal=38 |
Population: Intervention group (n=26): Mean age=51.4yr; Sex: males=9, females=17; Disease course: RRMS=22, PPMS=2, SPMS=2; Mean EDSS=5.5; Mean disease duration=16.3yr. Control group (n=12): Mean age=56.7yr; Sex: males=1, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=7, PPMS=2, SPMS=3; Mean/Median EDSS=5.6; Mean disease duration=21.4yr. Intervention: Participants in both groups received 15-20, 30-min sessions, 4-5x/wk for four weeks delivered by experienced physical therapists. The intervention group received treadmill dual-task training that was aimed at improving participant resistance, walking velocity, balance, and cognitive function. Each session consisted of an aerobic phase, a dual-task phase composed of motor and cognitive activities, and another aerobic phase. The intensity of the sessions was adjusted as the intervention sessions progressed. The control group received strength training aimed at strengthening muscles involved in walking. Exercise intensity was adjusted as the intervention progressed. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the 4-wk intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB).2. |
|
Veldkamp et al. 2019 Structured Cognitive-Motor Dual Task Training Compared to Single Mobility Training in Persons with Multiple Sclerosis, a Multicenter RCT Belgium, Italy, IsraelRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=47, NFinal=40 |
Population: Intervention group (n=20): Mean age =51.4yr; Sex: males=8, females=12; Disease course: RRMS=13, PPMS=3, SPMS=4; Mean EDSS=3.4; Mean disease duration=9.6yr. Control group (n=20): Mean age =53.4yr; Sex: males=9, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=13, PPMS=4, SPMS=3; Mean EDSS=3.7; Mean disease duration=11.4yr. Intervention: Following stratified randomization, both groups took part in an 8-wk program with 20 sessions. The intervention group completed the dual-task training protocol that consisted of exercises such as walking or stepping on the spot while completing 11 different cognitive tasks ranging in difficulty. The control group completed the single mobility training protocol which consisted of 21 different gait and dynamic balance exercises. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, after the intervention and at 4-wk f/u. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Dual Task Cost (DTC)1; Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N) (10/36-Spatial Recall Test (SPART),2 Word List Generation Test (WLGT),2 Selective Reminding Test (SRT),2 Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT),2 Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)).2. |
|
Sosnoff et al. 2017 Dual task training in persons with Multiple Sclerosis: a feasibility randomized controlled trial USARCT PEDro=7 NInitial=20, NFinal=14 |
Population: Intervention group (n=13): Mean age =48.3yr; Sex: males=8, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=7; Median EDSS=1.75; Mean disease duration=11.9yr. Control group (n=6): Mean age =56.8yr; Sex: males=1, females=5; Disease course: RRMS=5, PPMS=1; Median EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration=11.7yr. Intervention: Following randomization, participants took part in one of two, 12-wk interventions. The intervention group took part in dual-task training, which involves balance and gait training while simultaneously performing a cognitive task for half the session. The control group only completed the balance and gait training. Intensity was personalized to the participant's abilities. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 1wk following the end of the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BiCAMS)1 (California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)). |
|
Table 51. Summary Table of Studies Examining Cognitive-Motor Dual Task Training
Executive Function | Attention | Info processing | Memory | |
Improve | ||||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
In everyday life, carrying out motor and cognitive tasks simultaneously may be required. Three higher quality RCTs evaluated the effects of dual-task training protocols (cognitive task + physical training) compared to physical training alone. On testing of cognition outcomes alone (while not dual tasking), no statistically significant between-group differences were observed in any of the studies. Within-group analysis pre-post training in both the intervention and control groups for all three studies showed iether significant improvement or a trend towards improvement on the cognitive outcomes alone. Together, these results support that combining a cognitive task with motor training does not provide additional benefit on cognition testing when the cognitive testing occurs independent of the motor task. Not surprisingly, dual-task training did improve performance on dual-task outcomes for the intervention group (Veldkamp et al. 2019). Importantly, all three studies included in this section had active comparator groups involving strength training or gait and balance training. In comparison, Felippe et al. (2019) investigated specifically a balance exercise dual-task training protocol compared to a non-active control group and report significant improvement on executive function outcomes in favor of the dual-task intervention (See section 3.26.6). Dual-task training may have real-world relevance if improvements were transferable to dual-task activities in real-world setting (i.e., if cooking a meal was easier after dual task cognitive-motor training).
Conclusion
There is level 1a evidence that dual-task training does not improve information processing speed or memory more than balance or gait training alone (two randomized controlled trials; Sosnoff et al. 2007; Veldkamp et al. 2019).
There is level 1b evidence that dual-task training does not improve attention more than gait training alone (one randomized controlled trial; Veldkamp et al. 2019).
There is level 1b evidence that dual-task training does not improve executive function more than strength training (one randomized controlled trial; Jonsdottir et al. 2018).
Dual-Task Training combined with gait training may not improve attention, memory, or information processing speed more than gait training alone in persons with MS.
Dual-Task Training may not improve executive function more than strength training in persons with MS.
Exercise training may include many different types of exercise modalities, frequencies, and intensities. The literature on the effects of exercise training on cognition in MS continues to be an area of high research interest at the time of this module preparation. Importantly, in the dementia literature, exercise interventions may delay memory decline or improve cognition. Exercise interventions in early dementia (where there exists minimal CI at baseline) may be most effective for delaying cognitive decline (Cui et al. 2018; Du et al. 2018).
3.26.1 Aerobic and Strength Training
Previous studies have supported an association between exercise participation and improved cognitive performance in persons with MS (Robert W. Motl and Sandroff 2018; Angevaren et al. 2008; Engeroff, Ingmann, and Banzer 2018). Exercise guidelines recommend moderate-intensity aerobic exercises for 30 minutes and resistance training twice per week for people with mild and moderate MS (Latimer-Cheung et al. 2013). For people with moderate and severe MS, "as the disease progresses and engaging in exercise and physical activity becomes more challenging, referrals to specialists are essential for ensuring that patients' exercise and physical activity strategies are individualized to best meet their needs" (Rosalind Kalb et al. 2020, p.1461).
Table 52. Studies Examining Aerobics and Strength for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Coghe et al. 2018 Fatigue, as measured using the Modified Fatigue Impact Scale, is a predictor of processing speed improvement induced by exercise in patients with multiple sclerosis: data from a randomized controlled trial ItalyRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=30, NFinal=30 |
Population: Intervention group (n=11): Mean age=47.54yr; Sex: males=6, females=5; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3.68; Disease duration: unspecified.
Control group (n=11): Mean age=43.37yr; Sex: males=6, females=5; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Following randomization, the intervention group took part in a supervised, 24-wk program that included 3x60-min sessions/wk of aerobic and strength training sessions. Aerobic training involved cycle ergometer and gait training, and the strength training included exercises targeting upper limbs, lower limbs, and trunk. The control group could not be participating in any systematic physical activity or rehabilitation program. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, following the 6-mo intervention, and 6mo after completion. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment in MS (BICAMS)3 (Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)); Attention Network Test-Interaction (ANT-I); California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II)3; Brief Visual Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R).3. |
|
Sandroff et al. 2017 Multimodal exercise training in multiple sclerosis: A randomized controlled trial in persons with substantial mobility disability USARCT PEDro=5 NInitial=83, NFinal=62 |
Population: Intervention group (n=43): Mean age=49.8yr; Sex: males=7, females=36; Disease course unspecified; Mean PDDS=4.0; Disease duration: unspecified.
Control group (n=40): Mean age=51.2yr; Sex: males=5, females=35; Disease course unspecified; Mean PDDS=3.0; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Participants in the intervention group completed 3, 30-60-min supervised sessions/wk over 24wks. Participants spent equal time in every session on aerobic exercise, lower-extremity resistance, and balance training. The control group completed stretching and toning activities at the same frequency as the intervention group. Exercises were progressed through the course of the intervention for both groups. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, mid-point, and following the 6-mo intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT).3. |
|
Sandroff et al. 2019 Response heterogeneity in fitness, mobility and cognition with exercise-training in MS USARCT PEDro=4 NInitial=83, NFinal=32 |
Population: Mean age=49.8yr; Sex: males=7, females=25; Disease course unspecified; Median PDDS=4.0; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: The participants in the intervention group completed 3, therapist-lead training sessions/wk over 24wks. Sessions lasted between 30 and 60mins. Exercises focused on aerobic, balance and lower-extremity resistance training. The control group completed a stretching and toning program at the same frequency and duration. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and after the 6-mo intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT).3. |
|
Sangelaji et al. 2015 The effect of exercise therapy on cognitive functions in multiple sclerosis patients: A pilot study IranPre-Post NInitial=21, NFinal=17 |
Population: Mean age=37.1yr; Gender: males=3, females=14; Disease course: RRMS=15, SPMS=2; Mean EDSS=2.35; Mean disease duration: Unspecified. Intervention: Patients participated in 3 sessions/wk for an average number of 22.5 exercise sessions. The intervention consisted of aerobic exercise, balance, and resistance exercises. Outcome measures were completed prior to and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Selective Reminding Test (SRT): short term, long term; 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)-3, -2 min; Word List Generation Test (WLGT).3. |
|
Table 53. Summary Table of Studies Examining Aerobics and Strength
Improve | No statistical sig. difference | |
Information Processing |
|
|
Attention |
|
|
Memory |
|
|
⛨ | EDSS ≥ 5 or PDSS ≥ 3 |
↗ | Progressive MS |
〰 | Relapse-Remitting MS |
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
A limitation of the research investigating aerobic and strength training is that cognition was generally not impaired at baseline. A post hoc analysis of the RCT by Sandroff et al. (2017) supports a response heterogeneity in that participants with lower baseline cognitive processing speeds may be more likely to improve on cognitive outcomes after an exercise intervention (Sandroff et al. 2019). The pre-post study by Sangelaji et al. (2015) did include participants where the mean processing speed score on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test was impaired at baseline. Scores on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test in this study significantly improved post intervention by almost 4 points-which may be approaching a clinically meaningful change score. Results of this same study also report statistically significant improved scores on a memory test and on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)-3 (where new digits to add are presented every 3 seconds). However, scores did not improve signficantly on the PASAT-2 (digits presented every 2 seconds), a spatial memory test or a word list generation test.
A second limitation with all of the aerobic and strength training interventional studies is the short duration of the trials, with no intervention lasting longer than 24 weeks, and no follow-up after 6 months. None of the studies report adverse effects with the exercise interventions. The possible protective effects of adherence with a mixed strength and aerobic training program on cognitive decline in the longer-term specific to the MS population warrants further study.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that aerobic and strength training combined may not improve information processing speed in persons with MS at six-month follow-up (two randomized controlled trials; Coghe et al. 2018. Sandroff et al. 2017).
There is level 1b evidence that aerobic and strength training combined may not improve attention in persons with at six-month follow-up (one randomized controlled trial; Coghe et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that aerobic and strength training combined may not improve memory in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Coghe et al. 2018, Sangelaji et al. 2015).
Aerobic and strength training combined may not improve information processing speed, attention, or memory after short-term follow-up in MS.
Long-term effects and response heterogeneity warrant further study.
Due to balance impairment, muscle spasticity, and weakness, it may become difficult for some PwMS to safely ride a non-stationary classic bicycle. A variety of cycling adaptations for PwMS may allow cycling training. Types of accessible cycling options may include recumbent bicycles, tricycles, e-bikes, and hand-cycle ergometers.
Table 54. Studies Examining Cycling for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Baquet et al. 2018 Short-term interval aerobic exercise training does not improve memory functioning in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis-a randomized controlled trial GermanyRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=68, NFinal=57 |
Population: Intervention group (n=34): Mean age=38.2yr; Sex: males=13, females=21; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=1.7; Mean disease duration=8.1yr.
Control group (n=34): Mean age=39.6yr; Sex: males=9, females=25; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=1.8; Mean disease duration=9.1yr. Intervention: Participants were randomized to the intervention group or a waitlist control group. Following randomization, the intervention group completed individualized, physiotherapist-supervised, 12wks of bicycle ergometer-based aerobic exercise consisting of 2-3 sessions/wk. Each participant received a pre-definedtraining session schedule. Following the 12-wk intervention, participants in the control group were invited to complete the training and the intervention group was invited to continue training. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, after the intervention, and at the end of the extension phase. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Verbal Learning and Memory Test (VLMT)1; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)2; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)2; Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)2; Corsi Block-tapping Task (CORSI)2; Regensburger Verbal Fluency Test (RVWT).2. |
|
Briken et al. 2014 Effects of exercise on fitness and cognition in progressive MS: A randomized, controlled pilot trial GermanyRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=47, NFinal=42 |
Population: Arm ergometry group (n=10): Mean age=49.1yr; Gender: males=5, females=5; Disease course: SPMS=8, PPMS=2; Mean EDSS=5.2; Mean disease duration=17.1yr.
Rowing group (n=11): Mean age=50.9yr; Gender: males=4, females=7; Disease course: SPMS=7, PPMS=4; Mean EDSS=4.7; Mean disease duration=14.1yr.
Bicycle ergometry group (n=11): Mean age=48.8yr; Gender: males=5, females=6; Disease course: SPMS=8, PPMS=3; Mean EDSS=5.0; Mean disease duration=13.3yr.
Control group (n=10): Mean age=50.4yr; Gender: males=4, females=6; Disease course: SPMS=8, PPMS=2; Mean EDSS=4.9; Mean disease duration=18.9yr. Intervention: Participants were randomized to arm ergometry, rowing, bicycle ergometry, or a waitlist control condition. Participants completed an 8-10-wk intervention period, with 2-3 sessions/wk. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)2; Verbal Learning and Memory Test (VLMT)2; Test of Attentional Performance (TAP); Leistungsprüfsystem (LPS)2; Regensburger test of word fluency (RWT).2. |
|
Oken et al. 2004 Randomized controlled trial of yoga and exercise in multiple sclerosis USRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=69, NFinal=57 |
Population: Control group (n=20): Mean age=48.4yr; Gender: males=0, females=20; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=3.1; Mean disease duration: Unspecified.
Intervention (Yoga) group (n=22): Mean age=49.8yr; Gender: males=20, females=2; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=3.2; Mean disease duration: Unspecified.
Intervention (Bicycling) group (n=15): Mean age=48.8yr; Gender: males=2, females=13; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=2.9; Mean disease duration: Unspecified. Intervention: Participants were randomized to one of three groups: Yoga class, exercise class (bicycling), or waitlist control group. The classes were provided weekly for 6mos. Participants were assessed at baseline and at the end of the 6-mo period. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Stroop color and word test (SCWT)1; Cambridge Neurophysiological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB)-attentional shifting2; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)2; Stanford sleepiness scale (SSS); State-trait anxiety inventory (STAI). |
|
Sandroff et al. 2015 Acute effects of walking, cycling, and yoga exercise on cognition in persons with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis without impaired cognitive processing speed USARCT Crossover PEDro=5 NInitial=24, NFinal=24 |
Population: Mean age=44.2yr; Gender: males=1, females=23; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3.0; Mean disease duration=9.6yr. Intervention: MS patients underwent 4 experimental conditions consisting of 20min of moderate-intensity treadmill walking exercise, moderate-intensity cycle ergometer exercise, guided yoga, and quiet rest in a randomized, counterbalanced order. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 5min of completion of each experimental condition. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Modified-Flanker Task (FT): reaction time (RT), congruent trials, incongruent trials.1. |
|
Bahmani et al. 2019 In Patients with Multiple Sclerosis, Both Objective and Subjective Sleep, Depression, Fatigue, and Paresthesia Improved After 3 Weeks of Regular Exercise SwitzerlandPre-post NInitial=51, NFinal=46 |
Population: Mean age=50.74yr; Sex: males=10, females=36; Disease course unspecified; Mean EDSS=5.3; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Participants were patients at an inpatient rehabilitation centre. The intervention involved 5, 30-min sessions/wk for 3wks. The exercise program was a cycling program at 60rpm at the lactate threshold (75% of HRmax or 65% of VO2peak). The participants' regular rehabilitation program was continued simultaneously. Outcome and EEG measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)2; Symbol Digit Modality Test (SDMT).2 |
|
Barry et al. 2018 Impact of short-term cycle ergometer training on quality of life, cognition, and depressive symptomatology in multiple sclerosis patients: a pilot study IrelandPre-post NInitial=20, NFinal=19 |
Population: Multiple sclerosis group (n=9): Mean age=35.33yr; Sex: males=1, females=8; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.17; Mean disease duration=5.8yr. Healthy control group (n=10): Mean age=36yr; Sex: males=2, females=8. Intervention: All participants completed an 8-wk cycle ergometer, therapist-supervised program, consisting of 2 sessions/wk lasting 30min each at 65-75% age-predicted max heart rate. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the 8-wk training intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB)3: includes a motor screening, a mood rating scale, test of hippocampal-independent visuospatial memory, sustained attention, and executive function/cognitive flexibility. |
|
Swank, Thompson, and Medley 2013 Aerobic exercise in people with multiple sclerosis: its feasibility and secondary benefits USAPre-post NInitial=9, NFinal=6 |
Population: Mean age=42.7yr; Gender: males=2, females=7; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3; Mean disease duration=3.2yr. Intervention: MS patients performed a period of aerobic exercise followed by a period of unstructured physical activity. Aerobic exercise was performed for 30min (upper and lower body ergometry and treadmill ambulation) 2x/wk for 8wks. Assessments were performed at baseline (S1), post intervention (S18), and after 3mos of self-directed exercise (S19). Eight patients completed assessment at S18 and 6 completed S19 assessment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test 3 second (PASAT-3); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II).3 |
|
Table 55. Summary Table of Studies Examining Cycling
Information Processing | Attention | Memory | Executive Function | Verbal Fluency | |
Improve |
|
|
|
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
|
|
⛨ | EDSS ≥ 5 or PDSS ≥ 3 |
↗ | Progressive MS |
〰 | Relapse-Remitting MS |
Bold | PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | PEDro < 6 |
Italic | Non-RCT |
Discussion
There is conflicting evidence for the effect of a cycling intervention on cognitive outcomes between studies and across the cognitive domains. Possible moderating factors, with respect to the response to cycling exercise interventions on cognitive outcomes, may include timing of the cognitive testing in relation to the exercise, intensity of the exercise, MS disease course, degree of CI at baseline, physical deconditioning at baseline, and tolerance for the exercise intervention. Overall, there are more studies not supporting a benefit on cognition; however, most studies include people with RRMS without significant CI at baseline. Interestingly, in the protocol by Sandroff et al. (2015), RRMS participants without CI at baseline, when tested within five minutes of completing their exercise, improved on reaction time cognitive testing. Participants with progressive MS in the Briken et al. (2014) study also improved on reaction time testing (Test of Attentional Performance), but no improvement was observed in the domains of executive function, processing speed, or verbal fluency. In the Briken et al. (2014) study, the exact timing of the cognitive testing in relation to the exercise is not clear. It is possible that there are immediate effects of exercise on reaction time. Mechanisms for the improvement observed in reaction time post cycling exercise may include increased arousal and decreased symptoms of spasticity, which may temporarily affect performance on cognitive-motor reaction time-based tasks. Clinically, PwMS may wish to consider the timing of daily cognitive reaction time-based tasks in relation to their exercise routines.
Future research should include the exact timing of the exercise in relation to the cognitive testing, and details about the intensity of the training. Seated cycling may offer a safe exercise intervention for people at higher risk for falls if they transfer to a stationary bike. A cycle ergometer setup may be accessible from a wheelchair. Briken et al. (2014) report improved reaction time in participants with progressive MS even with a low-intensity cycling intervention. It is not clear if there may be additional benefits to cognition associated with higher intensity cycling training; this is perhaps dependent on baseline function and individual training responses (See also section 3.26.4 for high- versus moderate-intensity cycling training).
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that cycling may improve memory for persons with progressive MS (one randomized controlled trial; Briken et al. 2014).
There is level 1a evidence that cycling does not improve information processing speed compared to waitlist control in persons with MS (three randomized controlled trials; Baquet et al. 2018, Briken et al. 2014, and Oken et al. 2004).
There is level 1a evidence that cycling does not improve executive function for persons with MS (two randomized controlled trials; Briken et al. 2014, Oken et al. 2004).
There is level 1b evidence that cycling may not improve memory for persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial and two pre-post studies; Baquet et al. 2018, Barry et al. 2018, and Swank et al. 2013).
There is level 1b evidence that cycling may not improve verbal fluency for persons with progressive MS (one randomized controlled trial; Briken et al. 2014).
There is conflicting evidence whether cycling improves attention in persons with MS (two randomized controlled trials; Briken et al. 2014 and Oken et al. 2004).
There is conflicting evidence whether cycling improves cognition in persons with MS, with positive results for improving memory in persons with progressive MS.
People with multiple sclerosis may require adapted exercise therapy programs. A progressive start-to-run program in ambulatory PwMS included cognitive outcomes (Feys et al. 2019).
Table 56. Studies Examining Running for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Feys et al. 2019 Effects of an individual 12-week community located "start-to-run" program on physical capacity, walking, fatigue, cognitive function, brain volumes, and structures in persons with multiple sclerosis BelgiumRCT PEDro=4 NInitial=42, NFinal=35 |
Population: Intervention group (n=21): Mean age=36.6yr; Sex: males=1, females=20; Disease course unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=8.1yr.
Control group (n=21): Mean age=44.4yr; Sex: males=3, females=18; Disease course unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=9.2yr. Intervention: The participants in the intervention group were involved in a 12-wk start-to-run program. Participants trained 3x/wk and received an individualised training schedule. Training was designed for the goal of running 5km non-stop at 12wks. Participants also wore an activity tracker that required weekly upload. Group training sessions were organized at week 4 and 8 and were supervised by the researcher and master students. These sessions included education elements, individual knowledge acquisition, social interactions, etc. The control group was a waitlist control group that was offered a training program following the end of the study timeline. Both groups had the goal to run a public 5-km race. Outcome and MRI measures were collected at baseline and at 12wks. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N)2 (10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36;10/36-SPART; SPART); Word List Generation Test (WLGT), Selective Reminding Test (SRT); Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST)); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT).2. |
|
Huiskamp et al. 2020 A pilot study of the effects of running training on visuospatial memory in MS: A stronger functional embedding of the hippocampus in the default-mode network? NetherlandsRCT PEDro=3 NInitial=29, NFinal=29 |
Population: Intervention group (n=15): Mean age=38.1yr; Sex: males=0, females=15; Disease course unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=9.9yr.
Control group (n=14): Mean age=44.7yr; Sex: males=1, females=13; Disease course unspecified; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=8.8yr. Intervention: The participants in the intervention group completed a 12-wk, community based, start-to-run program, including sessions 3x/wk. The goal was to run a 5-km run. The control group consisted of a waitlist control. Outcome and fMRI measures were collected at baseline and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART); Selective Reminding Test (SRT).3. |
|
Table 57. Summary Table of Studies Examining Running
Memory | |||
Information Processing Speed | Spatial Memory | Verbal Learning and Memory | |
Improve |
|
||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
Bold | PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | PEDro < 6 |
Italic | Non-RCT |
Discussion
The Huiskamp et al. (2020) reports a subsample from the larger RCT by Feys et al. (2019) comparing a 12-week running program to a wait list control group. Therefore, these two manuscripts report overlapping samples of participants with MS. The interventional running group also received education, socialization, and individualized coaching, with the goal of running 5 kilometres at the end of the 12 weeks. In the original RCT by Fey et. al. (2019), primary outcomes included aerobic capacity and functional outcomes, while secondary outcomes included cognition and MRI brain volume and structural connectivity. The trial was positive in favor or the running group for improvement in VO2max, functional mobility, visual spatial memory, and increased volume of the pallidum on MRI. The manuscript by Huiskamp et al. (2020) elaborates on the fMRI outcomes through which researchers sought to explore the effects of running exercise on resting functional connectivity of the hippocampus, and on the default-mode network (DMN) on fMRI in relation to memory function. The DMN was defined as 38 cortical regions, spanning bilateral medial prefrontal areas, temporal and parietal regions, and posterior cingulate cortex.
Only the running group significantly improved post training on the 10/36 Spatial Recall Test (SPART). The SPART consists of a checkerboard with randomly placed checkers, and the participant must recall the placement of the checkers (Sousa et al. 2021). Functional connectivity of the DMN only significantly correlated with performance on the SPART in the intervention group post training (r=062; p=032) (Huiskamp et al. 2020). The mean age in intervention group, however, was over six years younger than the control group. After correcting for age, the improvement on the SPART observed in the running group was no longer statistically significant. Authors suggest that larger and longer studies exploring exercise intensity effects are warranted. The study sample included participants with a mean disease duration of approximately 10 years and a mean baseline Timed 25-Foot Walk Test of approximately 4.0 seconds (less than four seconds is normal walking speed). Participants also trained on an outdoor running track in a group setting, therefore experiencing three-dimensional visual input while training. Interestingly, participants in the running group improved on the SPART test, but not on the Selective Reminding Test, which involved recalling a list of 12 words. This trial supports the feasibility and safety of outdoor track running in people without significant walking impairment for a possible benefit on spatial memory.
Conclusion
There is level 2 evidence that running may improve spatial memory but not verbal learning and memory or information processing speed in persons with MS (two randomized controlled studies; Feyst et al. 2019; Huiskamp et al. 2020).
Preliminary evidence supports that running may improve spatial memory but not verbal learning and memory or information processing speed in persons with MS.
3.26.4 High-Intensity Aerobic Interval Training
High-intensity interval training (HIIT) typically involves short periods of maximal effort alternating with short rest periods. HIIT protocols may vary in their activation of anaerobic or aerobic pathways.
Table 58. Studies Examining High-Intensity Interval Training for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Zimmer et al. 2018 High-intensity interval exercise improves cognitive performance and reduces matrixmetalloproteinases-2 serum levels in persons with multiple sclerosis: A randomized controlled trial GermanyRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=61, NFinal=57 |
Population: Intervention group (n=27): Mean age=51yr; Sex: males=7, females=20; Disease course: RRMS=14, SPMS=13; Mean EDSS=4.27; Mean disease duration=11.98yr.
Control group (n=30): Mean age=48yr; Sex: males=12, females=18; Disease course: RRMS=16, SPMS=14; Mean EDSS=4.37; Mean disease duration=13.3yr. Intervention: Participants in the high-intensity interval training group completed 3 training sessions/wk for 3wks, with sessionsconsisted of 5x3-min intervals at 90% VO2peak. The control group completed 5 training sessions/wk for 3wks with 30min of continuous exercise at 65% VO2peak. Outcome measures and blood samples were collected at baseline and following the 3-wk intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment for Multiple Sclerosis (BiCAMS)1 (California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)); Trail Making Test (TMT-A, -B)1; Go/No Go tasks of the Test of Attention Performance (TAP).1. |
|
Table 59. Summary Table of Studies Examining High-Intensity Interval Training
Attention | Executive Function | General Cognitive | Information Processing Speed | Memory | |
Improve |
|
||||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
|
|
Bold | PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | PEDro < 6 |
Italic | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Zimmer et. al (2018) conducted a high-quality RCT comparing aerobic cycling at 80%VO2max compared to cycling at 60%VO2max. The study was sufficiently powered to detect small to moderate effect sizes on cognitive outcomes. Secondary outcomes included VO2peak and brain-derived neurotrophic factor, serotonin, and matrix metalloproteinase (MMP) 2 and -9 serum levels. Interaction effects (time×group) showed significant differences in favor of the HIIT group only for improved verbal memory and cardiorespiratory fitness, and a decrease in serum MMP-2 levels. Both groups improved pre-post training on other cognitive outcomes. Approximately 60% of participants in both groups had cognitive impairment on the BiCAMS on one of the three tests, and SDMT mean baseline scores were ~41.5 with no between-group differences. Importantly, this study excluded participants who were on any immunosuppressant, had a history of psychological disorders or had a severe cardiorespiratory condition, or had an EDSS score greater than 6 or less than 1. Thirty-six percent of participants screened met criteria for randomization. Once randomized, only 2 participants in the HIIT group and 1 participant in the moderate-intensity training group did not complete the protocol.
The results support that, for the activated patient without comorbid psychiatric conditions or severe cardiovascular disease, prescription of HIIT is feasible and may provide additional benefit on verbal memory outcomes compared to moderate-intensity exercise. Limitations of this study include the lack of a non-exercising control group, and the absence of details on how often during the training sessions the VO2 training targets were met for each group. VO2peak in both groups was similar at baseline (mean: 19 to 20 mL/kg/min), and VO2 improved to a mean of 23mL/kg/min only for the HIIT group. It is unclear if higher or lower VO2 peaks at baseline, or if larger changes in VO2 may affect cognitive outcomes over time; however, small to moderate correlations between increased VO2 and improved cognitive function at single time points exist.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that high-intensity aerobic cycling training may improve verbal memory compared to moderate-intensity aerobic cycling training in people with a baseline VO2 peak of ~20mL/kg/min (one randomized controlled trial; Zimmer et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that high-intensity aerobic cycling training may not improve attention, processing speed, or visual-spatial memory compared to moderate-intensity aerobic cycling training in people with a baseline VO2 peak of ~20mL/kg/min (one randomized controlled trial; Zimmer et al. 2018).
High-intensity aerobic training may improve verbal memory compared to moderate intensity aerobic training but may not improve cognition in other cognitive domains.
Circuit training involves performing different exercises with short rest periods in between each exercise. A set number of repetitions are completed, or circuit training may occur for prespecified amount of time.
Table 60. Studies Circuit Training for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Ozkul et al. 2020 Effect of task-oriented circuit training on motor and cognitive performance in patients with multiple sclerosis: A single-blinded randomized controlled trial TurkeyRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=23, NFinal=20 |
Population: Intervention group (n=10): Mean age=46yr; Sex: males=4, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=6, PPMS=4; Mean EDSS=4; Mean disease duration=16yr.
Control group (n=10): Mean age=41.5yr; Sex: males=4, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=6, PPMS=4; Mean EDSS=3.75; Mean disease duration=13.5yr. Intervention: Participants in both groups completed 2 sessions/wk for 6wks. The task-oriented circuit training group participated in 10 exercises in a variety of different settings and task difficulty was increased by altering sensory input (e.g. eyes closed, soft surface, etc.). The relaxation group were taught progressive relaxation exercises and were advised to practice them 2x/wk. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the 6-wk intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests (BRB-N)3 (10/36 Spatial Recall Test (10/36; 10/36-SPART; SPART), Word List Generation Test (WLGT), Selective Reminding Test-Long-Term (SRT), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test-3 (PASAT-3)); Multiple Sclerosis Neuropsychological Questionnaire (MSNQ).3. |
|
Table 61. Summary Table of Studies Examining Circuit Training
General Cognitive | Information Processing Speed | Memory | |
Improve | |||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
Bold | PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | PEDro < 6 |
Italic | Non-RCT |
Discussion
One RCT by Ozkul et al. (2020) includes the Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests as a tertiary cognitive outcome following a circuit training intervention. Significant improvement occurred pre-post training only in the circuit-training group on the Selective Reminding Test Long-Term, evaluating verbal and long-term memory. The control group received instruction to complete twice-a-week isometric muscle contraction and relaxation exercises while lying down (Jacobson's progressive relaxation exercises). The participants in the intervention group performed twice-a-week exercises involving 10 different motor tasks, each one for four minutes, with two minutes rest in between. Aerobic exercise intensity while circuit training is not provided. Balance was progressively challenged by alterations in the sensory input during the motor task by doing some tasks with eyes closed, while wearing sunglasses, while turning the head at the same time, or while standing on a softer surface. The study was powered to assess balance and walking outcomes, which significantly improved in favor of circuit training.
Ozkul et al. (2020) hypothesize why only memory improved significantly in the circuit training group, citing other work. Verbal memory tasks activate the premotor and supplementary motor cortex, and therefore motor training may be more likely to improve performance in verbal memory relative to other cognitive domains (Chein and Fiez 2001). Aerobic exercise is associated with increased hippocampal volume and improved performance on memory tasks in MS (Leavitt et al. 2014). There is the possibility of a spurious positive finding in the Ozkul et al. (2020) study for the Selective Reminding Test-Long-Term recall outcome-which was one of the eight different sub-tests of the Brief Repeatable Battery of Neuropsychological Tests. The Selective Reminding Test-Short-Term recall did not reach statistical significance for improvement in either group pre-post training, although effect sizes were larger in the circuit training group (d=0.51) compared to the control group (d= 0.34). The effects of relaxation on improving cognitive function in the control group may have diminished the power to detect significant between-group differences for any of the cognitive outcomes in this study. However, others have also shown selective improvement in verbal memory with aerobic cycling in progressive MS (Briken et al. 2014), and high-intensity interval training in MS (Zimmer et al. 2018), supporting that aerobic training may have a preferential impact on verbal memory function.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that circuit training may not improve memory, verbal fluency, visual processing speed, or auditory processing speed significantly more than relaxation exercises (one randomized controlled trial, Ozkul et al. 2020).
Preliminary evidence supports that circuit training may not improve memory, verbal fluency, or processing speed more than relaxation exercises in persons with MS
3.26.6 Balance Training and Dual Task
Dual-task balance training involves simultaneously performing a cognitive task and specifically a balance motor task together. The difficulty and the duration of the cognitive task and the balance task performed together may vary. However, different from motor training, the balance task challenges balance more than strength or exercise endurance.
Table 62. Studies Examining Balance Training and Dual Task for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Felippe et al. 2019 A Controlled Clinical Trial on the Effects of Exercise on Cognition and Mobility in Adults with Multiple Sclerosis BrazilRCT PEDro=7 NInitial=28, NFinal=27 |
Population: Intervention group (n=13): Mean age=35.0yr; Sex: males=10, females=3; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3.0; Mean disease duration=5.0yr.
Control group (n=14): Mean age=38.0yr; Sex: males=9, females=5; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.0; Mean disease duration=8.0yr. Intervention: Following randomization, the participants in the exercise group attended 2, 1-hr exercise sessions/wk for 6mos while the control group maintained their basic activities. The exercise intervention included activities targeting motor and cognitive function. The motor tasks included exercises for coordination and balance while the cognitive tasks included dual-task training. Outcome measures were collected at baseline, 3mos, and 6mos. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE)1; Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB).1. |
|
Table 63. Summary Table of Studies Examining Balance Training and Dual Task
Executive Function | General Cognitive | |
Improve |
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | PEDro < 6 |
Italic | Non-RCT |
Discussion
One small, high-quality RCT by Felippe et. al (2019) compared a very specific balance and cognitive training protocol to a sedentary control group. Participants were matched at baseline and fully ambulatory with a mean disease duration of less than 9 years. Results were significantly in favor of the intervention group for the two cognitive outcomes included (the Mini-Mental Satus Examination and the Frontal Assessment Battery) and the balance and dual-task outcomes. In addition, there was significant worsening for the balance outcomes and a trend towards worsening on the cognitive outcomes in the control group.
The intervention group received physiotherapist-guided, progressive balance and dual-task training. Participants completed 10-15 repetitive exercises for one hour twice a week. Exercises included using balls, rolls, dumbbells, and balance boards, as well as variable stepping exercises aimed to challenge balance and stimulate the core and upper and lower body muscle groups. While doing these exercises, participants completed cognitive tasks such as "sequencing, reasoning, attention, strategic planning, task shifting and memory" (Felippe et al. 2019, p. 99) Examples of the cognitive tasks include "defining nouns beginning with specific letters of the alphabet, circuits demanding sequential planning, recognizing, and reaching for objects with specific characteristics and naming fruits, animals, cities and countries." (Felippe et al. 2019, p. 99).
The unique aspect of this study is the targeted, combined progressive balance and cognitive dual-task training under the supervision of a physiotherapist. The results support a role for early physiotherapy intervention using progressive dual-task training in ambulatory PwMS who may not routinely use a mobility aid. This type of intervention may help improve and maintain both cognition and balance relevant to everyday mobility. A limitation of this study is that the cognitive evaluation of participants was limited to two cognitive outcomes (Mini-Mental Status Examination and Frontal Assessment Battery) and the validity of these outcomes in the MS population is not well established. A second limitation is the lack of an active control group. It is therefore unclear whether the cognitive tasks, the motor tasks, or the dual task training itself are driving the observed improvement in cognition. Cognitive-motor dual-task training in comparison to repetitive-motor or gait training alone may not provide additional benefit on cognitive outcomes (see section 3.25).
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that balance training combined with a dual task may improve general cognitive impairment and executive function at 6 months compared to no intervention in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial; Felippe et al. 2019).
Balance training coupled with dual task training may improve general cognition and executive function compared to no intervention in persons with relapsing-remitting MS.
Dancing is considered a multimodal therapy characterized by motor, cognitive, and motor-cognitive dual-task training (Hamacher et al. 2015; 2016). Partnered dance practices such as salsa and ballroom dance may also help with providing support in balance, movement, and fall prevention for the person living with MS (Ng et al. 2020).
Table 64. Studies Examining Dance for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Van Geel et al. 2020 Effects of a 10-week multimodal dance and art intervention program leading to a public performance in persons with multiple sclerosis - A controlled pilot-trial BelgiumPre-post NInitial=18, NFinal=17 |
Population: Dance group (n=7): Age range=29-52yr; Sex: males=0, females=7; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=3-21yr.
Art group (n=10): Age range=40-65yr; Sex: males=1, females=9; Disease course: RRMS; Severity: unspecified; Disease duration range=6-21yr. Intervention: Participants were allocated to groups based on preference. Both groups had 90-min sessions, 2x/wk over 10wks. The dance group received choreo-based dance therapy. The sessions included a 10-min warm up, 70-min training session, and 10-min cool down. The training included three choreographies of increasing difficulty. The art group included poem recitation and creating paintings, photography, and videography. The main goal for both groups was to work toward presenting their performance and work at an exhibition for an audience after the intervention. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 2wks after the live performance. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Modified Fatigue Impact Scale (MFIS)1; Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)2; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) 2; Dual Task: Word List Generation Test (WLGT) and subtraction.2 |
|
Ng et al. 2020 Ballroom dance for persons with multiple sclerosis: a pilot feasibility study USAPre-post NInitial=13, NFinal=13 |
Population: Intervention group (n=7): Mean age=49yr; Sex: males=1, females=6; Disease course: RRMS=6, PPMS=1; PDDS=2; Disease duration: unspecified.
Control group (n=6): Mean age=55yr; Sex: females=6; Disease course: RRMS; PDDS=2; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Participants were recruited from the National MS Society-Wisconsin Chapter. Individuals who could not participate due to scheduling or other commitments were placed in the control group. Dance sessions were 1hr in length and were hosted 2x/wk for 8wks, and participants were required to attend 6 of the 8wks. During the sessions, participants learned the rumba, foxtrot, waltz, and push-pull. The control group did not receive the dancing intervention but did complete all other procedures. Outcome measures were gathered 1wk before and following the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT).3 |
|
Table 65. Summary Table of Studies Examining Dance
Info processing | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | PEDro < 6 |
Italic | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Two non-randomized studies included secondary cognitive outcomes after a dancing intervention. Results are conflicting for significant improvement in processing speed on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT). However, the study by Van Geel et al. (2020) was close to reaching statistical significance for pre-post improvements on the PASAT in the dancing intervention group. The comparator group in this study was an active control group that received art therapy. Interestingly, the art therapy group improved significantly pre-post on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test, but only trended towards improvement on the PASAT. The Symbol Digit Modalities Test challenges processing speed for symbol recognition while the PASAT involves no visual symbol recognition. It is unclear if this pattern of improvement on cognitive testing is related to the type of training with either dance or art therapy.
Outcomes assessing memory and other cognitive domains were not included. Future research could include effect size calculations for different cognitive outcomes to help inform if there are task-specific training effects. All participants in the Van Geel et al. (2020) study also put on a live performance that participants enjoyed. In contrast, in the ballroom dancing study by Ng et al., participants in the control group received no active intervention. Ng et al. provide a between-group analysis in favor of ballroom dancing on the PASAT (Ng et al. 2020). Both studies found dancing to be enjoyable, safe, and highly feasible in ambulatory participants with MS. Further research is warranted on whether dancing consistently improves cognition, and for which cognitive domains.
Conclusion
There is conflicting evidence whether dance training improves information processing speed in persons with MS (two pre-post studies; Van Geel et al. 2020; Ng et al. 2019).
There is conflicting evidence whether dance training improves information processing speed in persons with MS.
Walking and cognitive dysfunction are common in MS as the disease progresses (Benedict et al. 2011; Motl, Sandroff, and DeLuca 2016). People living with MS may find walking challenging, and walking limitations may significantly affect quality of life. Participating in exercise training, including treadmill walking, is associated with improved walking and cognitive function in MS (B. M. Sandroff and Motl 2020). This search identified four walking interventional studies and one-stepping interventional study which provide more insight on the reported association between walking and cognitive function.
Table 66. Studies Examining Walking for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Sandroff, Johnson, and Motl 2017 Exercise training effects on memory and hippocampal viscoelasticity in multiple sclerosis: a novel application of magnetic resonance elastography USARCT PEDro=6 NInitial=8, NFinal=8 |
Population: Mean age=43.5yr; Sex: males=0, females=8; Disease course: RRMS; Median EDSS=3.0; Disease duration: unspecified. Intervention: Participants in the intervention group completed 3, 15 to 40-min sessions/wk for 12wks. The intervention involved supervised treadmill walking that progressively increased the difficulty over the course of the intervention. Heart rate monitors were used to ensure precision of exercise prescription. The control group was a waitlist control. They were offered the 12-wk intervention at the end of the study. Outcome and magnetic resonance elastography (MRE) measures were collected at baseline and after the 12-wk intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II).1. |
|
Sandroff et al. 2016 Systematically developed pilot randomized controlled trial of exercise and cognition in persons with multiple sclerosis USARCT PEDro=5 NInitial=10, NFinal=10 |
Population: Exercise group (n=5): Mean age=41.6yr; Gender: females=5; Disease course: RRMS; Median EDSS=3.0; Mean disease duration=11.4yr.
Control group (n=5): Mean age=44.2yr; Gender: females=5; Disease course: RRMS; Median EDSS=2.5; Mean disease duration=12.2yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to the treatment group or to a waitlist control group. The treatment group received progressive treadmill walking exercise training for 12wks. Assessments were performed at baseline and after treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Delis-Kaplan Executive Function (D-KEFS): correct sorts, description; Modified Flanker Task (FT): reaction time (RT), interference control of reaction time (IC RT).3. |
|
Sandroff et al. 2015 Acute effects of walking, cycling, and yoga exercise on cognition in persons with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis without impaired cognitive processing speed USARCT Crossover PEDro=5 NInitial=24, NFinal=24 |
Population: Mean age=44.2yr; Gender: males=1, females=23; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3.0; Mean disease duration=9.6yr. Intervention: MS patients underwent 4 experimental conditions consisting of 20min of moderate-intensity treadmill walking exercise, moderate-intensity cycle ergometer exercise, guided yoga, and quiet rest in a randomized, counterbalanced order. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 5min of completion of each experimental condition. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Modified-Flanker Task (FT): reaction time (RT), congruent trials, incongruent trials.1. |
|
Van Geel et al. 2020 Feasibility study of a 10-week community-based program using the WalkWithMe application on physical activity, walking, fatigue and cognition in persons with Multiple Sclerosis BelgiumPre-post NInitial=19, NFinal=12 |
Population: Median age=42.5yr; Sex: males=0, females=19; Disease course: RRMS=18, SPMS=1; Severity: unspecified; Median disease duration=7yr. Intervention: Participants completed a 10-wk WalkWithMe app-led intervention. Each participant performed baseline testing and met with a physical therapist at the outset of the intervention to define their goal for the intervention. The goal was defined based on the participant's answer to the question asking how long they want to be able to walk without interruptions. An individualized program was then inputted into the app. Participants completed at least 2 sessions/wk. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the 10-wk intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)2; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT)2, Cognitive Fatigability Index (CFI).2 |
|
Table 67. Summary Table of Studies Examining Walking
Improve | No statistical sig. difference | |
Verbal learning and Memory |
|
|
Information Processing Speed |
|
|
Executive Function |
|
|
〰 | Relapse-Remitting MS |
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Information processing speed is the most impaired cognitive domain in MS. However, no studies included people with significant impairment in processing speed at baseline. Both the small RCT by Sandroff et al. (2016) and the pre-post study by Van Geel et al. (2020) included persons with mean processing speed scores that were within the normal range at baseline on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Despite this, after a walking intervention both studies still observed large effect sizes for improvement on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Only the pre-post study was sufficiently powered to reach a statistically significant change in Symbol Digit Modalities Test scores (Van Geel et al. 2020). The research is limited to small studies led by the same authors (Brian M. Sandroff et al. 2015; 2016; B. M. Sandroff, Johnson, and Motl 2017). Larger studies with longer-term follow-up in people with CI at baseline are needed to determine if walking interventions may help improve processing speed or slow the rate of decline in processing speed in PwMS over time.
From a practical perspective, walking is a low-cost intervention that is safely feasible for the majority of PwMS over the early disease course. However, PwMS may perceive their walking to be effortful both physically and cognitively even when by direct observation the gait impairments may appear invisible (Knox et al. 2020). In counselling about walking among PwMS, the increased energy cost of walking and additional measures to improve walking safety and endurance should be considered (i.e., pacing and adaptive equipment). The many health benefits associated with walking support that in PwMS, maintaining and increasing safe walking in everyday life or for exercise should be priority. The impact of walking interventions on cognition in MS remains uncertain. However, importantly, worsening on cognitive outcomes is not reported in the walking intervention arms of the trials, while worsening of cognition was observed in at least one control group (Brian M. Sandroff et al. 2016).
Conclusion
There is conflicting evidence whether walking programs may improve information processing speed in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Sandroff et al. 2016 and Van Geel et al. 2020).
There is conflicting evidence whether walking programs improve executive function in persons with MS (two randomized controlled trial; Sandroff et al. 2016 and Sandroff et al. 2015).
There is level 1b evidence that walking programs may not improve verbal learning and memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one randomized controlled trial; Sandroff et al. 2017).
There is conflicting evidence whether walking programs improve information processing speed or executive function in persons with MS.
Walking Programs may not improve verbal learning and memory in persons with MS.
Stepping interventions include step training where the natural sequence of stepping with normal overground walking is disrupted intentionally. Step training may require additional cognitive demands.
Table 68. Studies Examining Stepping for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Hoang et al. 2016 Effects of a home-based step training programme on balance, stepping, cognition and functional performance in people with multiple sclerosis--a randomized controlled trial AustraliaRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=50, NFinal=44 |
Population: Intervention group (n=28): Mean age=53.4yr; Gender: males=7, females=21; Disease course: RRMS=15, PPMS=8, SPMS=5; Mean EDSS=4.1; Mean disease duration=11.6yr.
Control group (n=22): Mean age=51.4yr; Gender: males=5, females=17; Disease course: RRMS=11, PPMS=2, SPMS=7, unknown=2; Mean EDSS=4.2; Mean disease duration=13.4yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to either the intervention or control groups. The intervention group performed step training for at least 2x/wk for 30min for 12wks. Those in the control group continued their usual physical activity. Assessments were performed at baseline and within 7d of the completion of the program. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Trail Making Test A and B3; Timed Up and Go-Dual Task; Choice stepping reaction time1; Stroop stepping test1. |
|
Sebastiao et al. 2018 Home-based, square-stepping exercise program among older adults with multiple sclerosis: results of a feasibility randomized controlled study USARCT PEDro=6 NInitial=26, NFinal=25 |
Population: Intervention group (n=15): Mean age=63.8yr; Sex: males=2, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=14, SPMS=1; Median EDSS=3.75; Mean disease duration=21.9yr.
Control group (n=xx): Mean age=65.1yr; Sex: males=1, females=9; Disease course: RRMS=9, SPMS=1; Median EDSS=4.25; Mean disease duration=19.9yr. Intervention: Participants in the intervention group received a home-based, 12-wk square-stepping exercise program. Participants were provided with a mat for home-based practice, an instruction manual, a pedometer to track compliance, and a logbook to track exertion, feeling, enjoyment, and physical and mental fatigue. Session length was progressed from 10 to 30min. The control group completed a light-intensity stretching and strengthening program. Intensity was increased through the addition of more exercises. Both groups received weekly Skype calls and biweekly meetings with an exercise trainer. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and following the 12-wk period. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BiCAMS)3 (California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)). |
|
Table 69. Summary Table of Studies Examining Stepping
Info Processing | Executive Function | Memory | |
Improve | |||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Hoang et al. (2016) investigated a step exergame intervention to no intervention and Sebastiao et al. (2018) compared a mat square-stepping intervention to light-intensity stretching and strengthening. Both studies were smaller feasibility studies and neither study reports statistically significant between-group differences for the purely cognitive outcomes.
In the Hoang et al. (2016) study, the intervention group used a stepmania open-source software program to provide timed stepping rhythms synchronized to stimuli presented on a television screen. The primary outcome measures in this study were the Choice stepping reaction time and Stroop stepping test time. While these are measures of selective attention, they rely on motor-stepping function as well. Other cognitive outcomes included the Symbol Digit Modalities Test, the Trail Making Test A and B, and a dual motor-cognitive task (the Timed Up and Go Test while counting backwards from 100 by three). The Symbol Digit Modalities Test and Trail Making Tests A and B did not reach statistical significance for between-group differences; however, the results support task-specific training effects since outcomes involving stepping improved. The authors also note that participants with worse impairment at baseline were more likely to improve across multiple outcomes over the course of the study.
One of the main limitations of the Sebastiao et al. (2018) study was that participants in the control group were more cognitively impaired at baseline on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test. This limitation potentially further decreased the power for finding statistically significant between-group differences. However, importantly, effect sizes remained larger for greater improvement on the cognitive outcomes in the stepping interventional group compared to the control group.
Step training improves attention in stepping tasks and this improvement may be relevant to maintaining balance in everyday life activities. However, larger studies are needed to determine if step training significantly improves other objective cognitive outcomes.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that square stepping may not improve visual spatial memory, verbal memory, or visual processing speed significantly more than a light stretching and strengthening program (one randomized controlled trial; Sebastião et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that a stepping exergame program may not improve visual-spatial processing speed significantly more than usual physical activities (one randomized controlled trial; Hoang et al. 2016).
Preliminary evidence from small studies supports that stepping exercises may not improve cognitive outcomes in persons with MS compared to usual activity or light physical activity.
Pilates is widely regarded as a mind-body fitness practice in which practitioners are taught to focus on breath and posture while training core muscle stability, strength, and flexibility (Wells, Kolt, and Bialocerkowski 2012), often using specially designed resistance equipment. Pilates is increasingly used in rehabilitation programs, particularly those treating low back pain, where meta-analysis supports improvements in pain scores but not disability (Lim et al. 2011). Core theoretical principles taught in Pilates include activation of core stability musculature, concentration during exercise, control of posture and movement, precision of technique, flow or smooth transition between movements, and coordinated breath control (Wells, Kolt, and Bialocerkowski 2012). Several of these putative elements can be linked in theory with elemental cognitive processes, such as attentional control, somatosensory and interoceptive awareness, and autonomic functioning.
Table 70. Studies Examining Pilates for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Abasiyanik et al. 2020 The effects of Clinical Pilates training on walking, balance, fall risk, respiratory, and cognitive functions in persons with multiple sclerosis: A randomized controlled trial TurkeyRCT PEDro=4 NInitial=42, NFinal=33 |
Population: Intervention group (n=16): Mean age=42.5yr; Sex: males=4, females=12; Disease course: RRMS=14, SPMS=2; Mean EDSS=3.06; Mean disease duration=12.59yr. Control group (n=17): Mean age=48.24yr; Sex: males=6, females=11; Disease course: RRMS=14, SPMS=3; Mean EDSS=3.24; Mean disease duration=9.83yr. Intervention: Participants in both groups conducted 3 sessions/wk for 8wks. The Pilates group was offered 1 session/wk of group exercises and 2 home-exercise sessions. The first session included education on abdominal draw-in maneuver and basic Pilates principles. Each session of 55-60min included exercises in different positions and use of resistance bands and exercise balls. Difficulty was increased gradually by upgrading the resistance bands. The control group home-exercise programs focused on flexibility, strength, trunk and pelvic stability, and balance. Progression included increased repetitions, change in positions, and decrease in base support. Weekly phone calls assessed compliance. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and at the end of the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment for MS (BiCAMS) (California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R)).1. |
|
Küçük et al. 2016 Improvements in cognition, quality of life, and physical performance with clinical Pilates in multiple sclerosis: a randomized controlled trial TurkeyRCT PEDro=4 NInitial=37, NFinal=20 |
Population: Pilates group (n=11): Mean age=47.2yr; Gender: males=4, females=7; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=3.2; Mean disease duration=14.8yr. Control group (n=9): Mean age=49.7yr; Gender: males=3, females=6; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=2.8; Mean disease duration=14.2yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomized to a Pilates group or an active control. The control group received a traditional exercise program including strength, balance, and coordination exercises. Both groups received 2d of exercise training/wk for 8wks and each session was 45-60min long. Assessments were performed before and after 8wks of treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Modified fatigue impact scale (MFIS), cognitive subscale.3. |
|
Kara et al. 2017 Different types of exercise in Multiple Sclerosis: Aerobic exercise or Pilates, a single-blind clinical study TurkeyPre-post NInitial=76, NFinal=56 |
Population: Aerobic exercise group (n=26): Mean age=43.03yr; Sex: males=9, females=17; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3.2; Mean disease duration=12.3yr. Pilates group (n=9): Mean age=49.77yr; Sex: males=3, females=6; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.85; Mean disease duration=14.22yr. Healthy control group (n=21): Mean age=44.42yr; Sex: males=8, females=13. Intervention: The MS participants were assigned to either the aerobic or Pilates exercise groups, while the healthy participants were assigned to the control group. Both groups were assigned to 2 sessions/wk for 8wks that were supervised by a physical therapist. The aerobic group was educated on how to monitor their heart rate and conducted a 30-40-min session of an aerobic exercise. The Pilates group was taught key elements of the practice including breathing, focus, and body placement. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and after the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Multiple Sclerosis Functional Composite (MSFC) (Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT-3).3 |
|
Table 71. Summary Table of Studies Examining Pilates
Info Processing speed | Memory | |
Improve |
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Two lower quality RCTs and one pre-post study included objective cognitive outcomes following a Pilates intervention, with improvement observed on the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test for all three studies.
Exercise is a core part of rehabilitation programs for PwMS and has several important benefits, including possibly playing a role in preventing disability progression through disease modifying effects (Dalgas et al. 2019), symptom management (R.W. Motl and Gosney 2008), and quality of life (Lara A. Pilutti et al. 2013). A recent meta-analysis reported a null effect of exercise interventions on cognitive function (Gharakhanlou et al. 2021). Since exercise modality, duration, and intensity may vary widely, the MSBEST module has summarized different exercise interventions separately. Caution is required when generalizing findings to a given patient or exercise modality. In meta-analysis, multimodal exercise is associated with upregulation of neuroplasticity biomarkers in PwMS (Diechmann et al. 2021), as has also been observed in a single Pilates study (Eftekhari and Etemadifar 2018).
Pilates is a form of exercise widely used by people with multiple sclerosis (Sánchez-Lastra et al. 2019), who perceive it as an accessible modality. PwMS report physical, psychological, social, and functional benefits following Pilates training, perceiving these benefits to be derived from increased awareness of core musculature in functional task performance (van der Linden et al. 2014). A recent systematic review of Pilates includes 10 RCTs and suggests multiple benefits (improvements in physical functioning, balance, mobility, fatigue, and quality of life) from individual studies, but meta-analysis found only marginal benefits when compared with active comparator conditions (Sánchez-Lastra et al. 2019).
Only two RCTs met the MSBEST criteria for objective cognitive outcomes following a Pilates intervention; however, both studies included active exercise comparator groups. In both RCTs, the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test, a measure of processing speed requiring sustained attention, improved significantly more in the Pilates group. It is feasible that Pilates may provide training in attentional control especially relevant to testing that demands sustained attention.
Conclusion
There is level 2 evidence that Pilates may improve information processing speed compared to traditional exercise programs in persons with MS (two randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Abasiyanik et al. 2020, Küçük et al. 2016, and Kara et al. 2017).
There is level 2 evidence that Pilates may improve memory in persons with MS compared to traditional exercise programs (one randomized controlled trial; Abasiyanik et al. 2020).
Preliminary evidence supports that Pilates may improve information processing speed and memory for persons with MS.
Yoga is an ancient Indian practice incorporating breathing, physical postures, and relaxation. Yoga might be classified into different types such as Hatha and Kundalini Ashtanga, which are gentle and focus on breath and pose, and Vinyasa, which is more physical (Senders et al. 2012). Although not all yoga practices are appropriate for PwMS, yoga's focus on breath, movement, and stretching may have potential to improve self-efficacy, mental health, and quality of life for PwMS (Frank and Larimore 2015).
Table 72. Studies Examining Yoga for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Oken et al. 2004 Randomized controlled trial of yoga and exercise in multiple sclerosis USRCT PEDro=6 NInitial=69, NFinal=57 |
Population: Intervention (Yoga) group (n=22): Mean age=49.8yr; Gender: males=20, females=2; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=3.2; Mean disease duration: Unspecified. Intervention (Exercise) group (n=15): Mean age=48.8yr; Gender: males=2, females=13; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=2.9; Mean disease duration: Unspecified. Control group (n=20): Mean age=48.4yr; Gender: males=0, females=20; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=3.1; Mean disease duration: Unspecified. Intervention: Participants were randomized to one of three groups: yoga class, exercise class (bicycling), or waitlist control group. The classes were provided weekly for 6mos. Participants were assessed at baseline and at the end of the 6-mo period. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT); Cambridge Neurophysiological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT).3. |
|
Sandroff et al. 2015 Acute effects of walking, cycling, and yoga exercise on cognition in persons with relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis without impaired cognitive processing speed USARCT Crossover PEDro=5 NInitial=24, NFinal=24 |
Population: Mean age=44.2yr; Gender: males=1, females=23; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=3.0; Mean disease duration=9.6yr. Intervention: MS patients underwent 4 experimental conditions consisting of 20min of moderate-intensity treadmill walking exercise, moderate-intensity cycle ergometer exercise, guided yoga, or quiet rest in a randomized, counterbalanced order. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and within 5min of completion of each experimental condition. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Modified-Flanker Task (FT) sub-scores: reaction time (RT), congruent trials, incongruent trials.1 |
|
Velikonja et al. 2010 Influence of sports climbing and yoga on spasticity, cognitive function, mood, and fatigue in patients with multiple sclerosis SloveniaRCT PEDro=5 NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: Sports Climbing group (SC; n=10): Median Age=42yr; Gender: Unspecified; Disease course: RRMS, PPMS, SPMS; Median EDSS=4; Disease duration: Unspecified. Yoga group (YG; n=10): Median Age=41yr; Gender: Unspecified; Disease course: RRMS, PPMS, SPMS; Median EDSS=4.2; Disease duration: Unspecified. Intervention: Participants with MS were randomly assigned to sports climbing exercise or yoga exercise for 10wks. Outcomes were assessed at baseline and post treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Tower of London Test (TOL): total number of moves (TOLtnm), total time (TOLtt); Brickenkamp d2Test (Bd2T); Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB): mazes subtest.3 |
|
Table 73. Summary Table of Studies Examining Yoga
Attention | Executive Function | |
Improve |
|
|
No statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Each of the three studies examining cognition following a yoga intervention included different cognitive outcomes. Only two of the three studies reported positive findings for only some of the cognitive outcome or subscores within these outcomes (Velikonja et al. 2010, Sandroff et al. 2015). Although yoga did not consistently improve executive function scores, yoga may help improve attention (Velikonja et al. 2010). The timing of the testing might influence results in the attention domain. Sandroff et al. (2015) report improved reaction time with testing completed within five minutes of ending the yoga session. Reaction time was evaluated as a subscore within the modified Flanker task, a test of executive function with attention components within. These three studies with a yoga intervention did not report any adverse effects. Although yoga may not provide a long-term benefit to cognition on objective testing, yoga may provide other health benefits to people living with MS. It would be rational to consider scheduling yoga exercise prior to completing daily tasks that require increased attentional demands. The studies did not include people with more advanced physical or cognitive disability. Modified seated yoga alternatives exist for people who are at risk for falls or unable to transfer independently to the floor. Modified yoga programs have not been studied for their effect on objective outcomes of attention in advanced MS.
Conclusion
There is conflicting evidence whether yoga improves executive function in persons with MS (two randomized controlled trials; Oken et al. 2004, Velikonja et al. 2010)
There is level 2 evidence that yoga may improve attention more than sports climbing in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Velikonja et al. 2010)
Preliminary evidence supports that yoga may improve attention in persons with MS.
There is conflicting evidence whether yoga improves executive function in persons with MS.
3.27.1 Functional Electrical Stimulation Cycling
Functional electrical stimulation (FES) cycling is a form of exercise training in which external surface electrodes over the quadriceps, hamstrings, and gluteal muscle groups are controlled by a microprocessor and custom software to create the leg cycling movement (Pilutti et al. 2019). We include FES cycling in this section of the module since it uniquely inovles external electrical stimulation compared to other modes of exercise discussed in section 3.26.
Table 74. Studies Examining Muscle Electrical Stimulation for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Pilutti et al. 2019 Functional Electrical Stimulation Cycling Exercise in People with Multiple Sclerosis Secondary Effects on Cognition, Symptoms, and Quality of Life USARCT PEDro=5 NInitial=11, NFinal=8 |
Population: Intervention group (n=4): Mean age =57.3yr; Sex: males=1, females=3; Disease course: RRMS=2, PPMS=2; Median EDSS=6.25; Mean disease duration=22.3yr.
Control group (n=4): Mean age=48.5yr; Sex: females=4; Disease course: RRMS=2, PPMS=2; Median EDSS=6.25; Mean disease duration=20.8yr.
Intervention: Participants were randomly allocated to the functional electrical stimulation (FES) or the passive leg cycling condition for 3 weekly sessions for 24wks. Both protocols were delivered at 50 rpm cadence, using the same training facility, equipment, and research staff. The intervention group received stimulation and active pedaling to the target cadence and prescribed heart rate as outlined by the American College of Sports Medicine and MS-specific physical activity guidelines for moderate to vigorous aerobic exercise. The control group did not receive any stimulation or active pedaling. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and at the end of the program Cognitive Outcome Measures: Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT).2 |
|
Table 75. Summary Table of Studies Examining Muscle Electrical Stimulation
Information Processing Speed | |
Improve |
|
No statistical sig. difference |
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Pilluti et al. (2019) reported secondary cognitive outcomes on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test from a pilot study comparing passive versus functional electrical stimulation (FES) cycling in MS. The original pilot study published elsewhere found FES cycling to be feasible and effective for improving mobility and cardiovascular fitness in ambulatory PwMS with more advanced disability (EDSS score 5.5 to 6.5) (Edwards et al. 2018). Encouragingly, those in the FES cycling group improved on the SDMT by 14.5% (a mean change score of 6 points) compared to 2.9% (a mean change score of 1.3 points) in the passive cycling group. This improvement in favor of FES cycling supports a likely clinically meaningful change. However, between-group statistical analyses are not provided due to the pilot nature of this study.
The FES cycling protocol involved a biphasic symmetrical waveform, a phase duration of 250 µs, and a pulse rate of 50 pulses per second. The FES cycle product used in this study was the RT300 cycle (Restorative Therapies Inc, Baltimore, MD). In the FES cycling group, target cycling cadence and prescribed heart rate were based on recommendations for moderate to rigorous aerobic exercise from the American College of Sports Medicine and MS-specific physical activity guidelines (American College of Sports Medicine 2013; Latimer-Cheung et al. 2013). In the passive cycling group, the cycle ergometer motor generated passive leg movement. A limitation with FES cycling in practice may be access to an FES cycling machine and staff familiar with FES cycling protocols. However, FES cycling does provide a safe platform for moderate to vigorous aerobic exercise in patients with restricted mobility, with potential for benefiting cognition in addition to other health benefits.
Conclusion
There is level 2 evidence that functional electrical stimulation cycling may improve visual processing speed compared to passive cycling in persons with MS with mobility impairments (one randomized controlled trial; Pilutti et al., 2019).
Functional electrical stimulation cycling may improve visual processing speed compared to passive cycling in persons with MS with mobility impairments.
3.27.2 High-Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
High-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) provides ≥5 Hz of repetitive magnetic stimulation (rTMS), against the scalp, inducing the excitability of a particular cortical region (Hulst et al. 2017). Health Canada first approved rTMS for the treatment of treatment-resistant major depressive disorder (Downar, Blumberger, and Daskalakis 2016). The Canadian Stroke Best Practice Guidelines recommend that rTMS could be considered as an adjunct to upper-extremity therapy (Evidence Level A) (Canadian Best Stroke Practices, 2019), but does not provide recommendations on the use of rTMS for language or perceptual impairments following stroke.
Table 76. Studies Examining High-Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Hulst et al. 2017 rTMS affects working memory performance, brain activation and functional connectivity in patients with multiple sclerosis NetherlandsRCT Crossover PEDro=7 NInitial=30, NFinal=28 |
Population: MS participants (n=17): Mean age=43.3yr; Gender: males=7, females=10; Disease course: RRMS=13, SPMS=4; Mean EDSS=3.5; Mean disease duration=11.9yr. Healthy controls (n=11): Mean age=42.3yr; Gender: males=5, females=6.
Intervention: MS patients and healthy controls underwent three experimental sessions (baseline, rTMS, sham-rTMS) including an N-back task under three load conditions: 1-back (N1), 2-back (N2), 3-back (N3), and 0-back control (N0). Assessments were performed at baseline and after rTMS or sham-rTMS. Cognitive Outcome Measures: N-back working memory task: accuracy, reaction time.1 |
|
Table 77. Summary Table of Studies Examining High-Frequency Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
Memory | |
Improve | |
No statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
One study reported on the effects of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on working memory and fMRI outcomes.(Hulst et al. 2017). Healthy controls and PwMS completed, in a random order, a visuospatial N-back working-memory task with three levels of increasing difficulty (1-back, 2-back, and 3-back) and a control condition (N0) while fMRI data was collected under the rTMS and sham-rTMS conditions. There was no statistically significant difference between the N-back scores in the rTMS and the sham-rTMS conditions. This small study may not have been sufficiently powered to detect between group differences. The authors do not include other outcome measures assessing memory or other cognitive domains. However, PwMS and the healthy controls in this study are reported to have normal cognitive function at baseline. For the PwMS, within the rTMS condition, results on the 2-back and 3-back accuracy test scores and 3-back reaction time scores significantly improved compared to the baseline scores. On fMRI, PwMS also displayed higher task-related frontal activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex compared to controls on the level 2 difficulty (2-back) of the N-back test. The increased activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex likely indicates abnormal and less-efficient brain connectivity in PwMS while performing a more difficult cognitive task. This finding of a less efficient brain activation pattern in PwMS is also supported by the work of others (Staffen et al. 2002). The increased cortical activation in the PwMS in the RCT by Hulst et al. (2017) interestingly normalized only after rTMS, but not after sham-rTMS.
A limitation of the Hulst et al. study is the strict inclusion criteria to safeguard against the risk of rTMS triggering a possible seizure in participants more susceptible to seizures. Exclusion criteria included: use of medication that lowers the seizure threshold (which presumably would indicate that participants on baclofen were excluded); and/or if participants had ≥12 cortical MS lesions. The study rTMS protocol is described as follows: 10 Hz, 110% RMT, 60 trains of 5 seconds, 25 seconds between trains, in total 3000 biphasic pulses in 30 minutes. The rTMS is individually positioned for each participant to ensure stimulation over the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The sham protocol differs in intensity with a lower intensity of 80% RMT, and in positioning over a non-effective area (2 centimetres posterior to the vertex). Authors suggest that rTMS should be explored in terms of safety for PwMS with more severe cognitive impairment and extensive MS lesion load.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) may not significantly improve working memory compared to sham-rTMS in persons with MS without cognitive impairment at baseline (one randomized controlled trial; Hulst et al. 2017).
Preliminary evidence from small studies supports that high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation may not improve working memory in persons with MS.
3.27.3 Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation
Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) involves a low- intensity direct current applied through surface electrodes placed on the scalp. The current modulates the membrane polarity of the underlying neurons thought to promote cortical excitability and neuroplasticity (Mattioli et al. 2016). The Canadian Best Stroke Practices Guidelines recommend tDCS "could be considered as adjunct therapy following stroke to improve upper limb function (Level B evidence)" (Canadian Stroke Best Practices 2019, p.38). However, these guidelines do not provide recommendations concerning tDCS for the rehabilitation of language or perceptual deficits following stroke. The exact therapeutic mechanisms are not well understood.
Table 78. Studies Examining Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation for Cognitive Impairment in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Mattioli et al. 2016 Neuroenhancement through cognitive training and anodal tDCS in multiple sclerosis ItalyRCT PEDro=8 NInitial=20, NFinal=20 |
Population: a-tDCS group (n=10): Mean age=38.2yr; Gender: males=3, females=7; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.1; Mean disease duration=6.6yr. Sham group (n=10): Mean age=47.2yr; Gender: males=1, females=9; Disease course: RRMS; Mean EDSS=2.9; Mean disease duration=11.0yr. Intervention: MS patients were randomly assigned to receive cognitive training with actual or sham anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (a-tDCS). a-tDCS was applied directly over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). The cognitive training involved combining attention training with real or sham a-tDCS for 10 daily sessions. Assessments were performed at baseline (T0), after treatment (T1), and 6mos later (T2). Cognitive Outcome Measures: Selective Reminding Test: long-term storage (SRT-LTS), consistent long-term retrieval (SRT-CLTR), delayed recall (SRT-D); Spatial Recall Test: delayed recall (SPART-D); Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT); Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task: 3, 2 seconds (PASAT-3, -2); Word List Generation Test (WLGT); Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST): total errors, perseverative responses, perseverative errors, non-perseverative errors.3. |
|
Chalah et al. 2017 Effects of left DLPFC versus right PPC tDCS on multiple sclerosis fatigue FranceCrossover RCT Design PEDro=6 NInitial=12, NFinal=10 |
Population: Mean age=40.5yr; Sex: males=6, females=4; Disease course: RRMS=9, SPMS=1; Mean EDSS=2.3; Mean disease duration=14.0yr. Intervention: Participants were randomized into three anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) blocks: active stimulation over left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), active stimulation over the right posterior parietal cortex (PPC), or sham stimulation over either cortical site. Participants were then assigned to a secondary active stimulation (e.g.: DLPFC assigned to PPC) or the sham protocol. Finally, participants were then crossed over to the opposite allocation (sham or active). Each participant received five consecutive daily sessions at the same time of day. There was a 3-wk washout period before outcome measures were collected on day 1 and day 5 of the intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Attention Network Test (ANT).3. |
|
Charvet et al. 2018 Remotely Supervised Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation Increases the Benefit of At-Home Cognitive Training in Multiple Sclerosis USAPre-post NInitial=46, NFinal=45 |
Population: Intervention group (n=25): Mean age=52.7yr; Sex: males=4, females=21; Disease course: RRMS=7, other=18; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=17.7yr. Control group (n=20): Mean age=51.0yr; Sex: males=7, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=15, Other=5; Severity: unspecified; Mean disease duration=15.7yr. Intervention: Participants were assigned to either the control group or the active intervention group. Both received 10, 20-min sessions over 2wks. The intervention group received transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and cognitive training. The tDCS was an at-home, technician-guided intervention that delivered 1.5mA of stimulation while the participants completed the cognitive training. Both groups received the cognitive training program, which focused on five areas: n-back, auditory span, visual span, simple arithmetic, and match-to-sample. Outcome measures were collected at baseline and post-intervention. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Brief International Cognitive Assessment in MS (BiCAMS) (Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)); Attention Network Test-Interaction (ANT-I); Cogstate Brief Battery.3. |
|
Table 79. Summary Table of Studies Examining Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation
Executive function | Info processing | Attention | Memory | |
Improve |
|
|
||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
Three studies tested the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on objective cognitive outcomes. Two of the three studies report positive findings favoring treatment for some of the cognitive outcomes (Mattioli et al. 2016; Charvet et al. 2018). Interestingly, the third study by Chalah et al. (2017) does not report any significant cognitive outcomes, but does report that tDCS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is associated with improved fatigue on the Fatigue Severity Scale and the Modfied Fatigue Impact Scale physical and psychological sub-scales, but not with the Modified Fatigue Impact Scale cognitive subscore. The primary outcome in the Chalah et al. (2017) study was fatigue.
In the RCT by Mattioli et al. (2016) and the prep-post study by Charvet et al. (2018), both the intervention and sham control groups participated in cognitive rehabilation exercises while undergoing tDCS or sham treatment, while the participants in the RCT by Chalah et al. (2017) did not. Possibly, tDCS enhances cognition on some outcome measures only when combined also with cognitive rehabilitation training.
Cognitive outcomes in both the sham and the treatment groups improved in all three studies, demonstrating the importance of having a sham control. The Mattioli et al. (2016) study report statistically significant findings for between-group differences for select cognitive outcomes (i.e., the Symbol Digit Modalities Test and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) and Charvet et al. (2018) report significant between-group differences for only one of their cognitive outcomes evaluating complex attention. The clinical significance of the positive between-group findings favoring the tDCS treatment on these select cognitive outcomes is uncertain. Effect size calculations in future tDCS research would be informative.
Mattioli et al. (2016) suggest that cognitive training during tDCS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex improves information processing speed and executive function through longer-term changes in the synaptic strength of the transmissions. Cognitive outcomes in the memory domain did not significantly improve after tDCS treatment compared to the sham treatment. However, the cognitive rehabilitation training protocols (for the two studies including cognitive exercises) did not focus specifically on memory-related training (Mattioli et al. 2016; Charvet et al. 2018).
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation combined with cognitive training may improve executive function compared to sham treatment combined with cognitive training (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Chalah et al. 2017, Charvet et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation may not improve basic attention compared to sham treatment (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Chalah et al. 2017, Charvet et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation may not improve memory in persons with MS compared to sham treatment (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Mattioli et al. 2016; Charvet et al. 2018).
There is conflicting evidence whether transcranial direct current stimulation combined with cognitive training improves visual information processing compared to sham treatment combined with cognitive training (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Mattioli et al. 2016, Charvet et al. 2018).
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex may improve executive function when combined with cognitive training tasks.
3.27.4 Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation
Transcranial random noise stimulation is a form of transcranial electric stimulation that involves transmission of a randomly oscillating current in a defined threshold over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Palm et al. 2016).
Table 80. Studies Examining Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation for CI in Multiple Sclerosis
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Palm et al. 2016 Effects of transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) on affect, pain, and attention in multiple sclerosis FranceRCT Crossover PEDro=5 NInitial=16, NFinal=16 |
Population: Mean age=47.4yr; Gender: males=3, females=13; Disease course: RRMS=11, SPMS=4, PPMS=1; Mean EDSS=4.2; Mean disease duration=12.5yr. Intervention: MS patients received two blocks of transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in a randomized order. The blocks were separated by a 3-wk washout period. Each block consisted of three consecutive daily sessions of either active or sham tRNS. Assessments were performed at baseline and after each tRNS block. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Attention Network Test (ANT); ANT subscales included: mean response time (MRT), mean accuracy (MA), alerting, orienting, executive control.3. |
|
Table 81. Summary Table of Studies Examining Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation
Attention | |
Improve | |
No statistical sig. difference |
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
One crossover RCT investigated the effects of transcranial random noise stimulation on neuropathic pain and attention in participants with MS (Palm et al. 2016). Participants received two blocks of transcranial random noise stimulation followed by a three-week washout period. The blocks involved either active or sham transcranial random noise stimulation that were delivered over three consecutive daily sessions. The sessions using transcranial random noise stimulation involved wearing a cap with predefined localization over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. The stimulation intensity and duration were set for 2mA and 20-30 minutes, respectively. The sham protocol involved the software ramping up for 15 seconds before it switched off. Attention was measured at baseline and following each training block. Results of the study concluded that there were no significant changes in attention following sham or transcranial random noise stimulation. A trend for improved pain was observed in the intervention group in this small study of 16 participants. The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has a role in pain and attention circuits, and authors suggest that protocols with longer stimulation may yield better outcomes.
Conclusion
There is level 1b evidence that transcranial random noise stimulation may not improve attention in persons with MS compared to placebo (from one randomized controlled trial; Palm et al. 2016).
Transcranial random noise stimulation may not improve attention in persons with MS.
3.27.5 Tongue Electrical Stimulation
The Portable Neuromodulation Stimulator (PoNSTM) is a medical device that provides direct transcutaneous electrical stimulation to the tongue, marketed by Helius Medical Technologies. The FDA approved the device in March of 2021 through their breakthrough device program as a novel medical device (FDA 2022; 2021a). According to the FDA, the PoNSTM is be used only as an adjunct to short-term, physiotherapist-guided gait training in people with MS with mild to moderate symptoms (FDA 2021b). Prior to this, Health Canada approved the device in 2018, the PoNSTM device and corresponding training is available through authorized PoNSTM therapy clinics. Cost varies from $20,000 to $30,000 CAD from site to site and includes the cost of the device ($9,500 CAD) and the cost of therapy sessions. CAD) and the cost of therapy sessions.
Data from two studies with 20 and 14 PwMS respectively (Tyler et al 2014, Leonard et. al. 2017), as well as real-world safety data, led to FDA approval of PoNSTM. The participants in the RCTs received real or sham tongue stimulation, and all participants in both studies received individualized, physiotherapist-guided, in-lab training for 90 minutes twice a day for 14 days, followed by home exercise training for an additional 12 weeks. Participants treated with sham or real tongue stimulation both improved over the course of these studies on the primary outcomes: the clinician-scored Dynamic Gait Index (Chiu et al. 2006) or the Sensory Organization Tasks (Broglio et al. 2008). The Sensory Organization Tasks consist of computerized balance data captured through a force plate. There was significantly greater improvement in favour of the intervention stimulation groups on the Dynamic Gait Index for the larger of the two studies (Tyler et al. 2014) and on the Sensory Organization Tasks for the smaller study (Leonard et al. 2017). More conventional gait outcomes utilized in MS, such as the Timed-25 Foot Walk Test, were not included. Authors propose that electrical stimulation of the tongue modulates central nervous system structures controlling balance and movement (Tyler et al. 2014).
Author Year Title Country Research Design PEDro Sample Size |
Methods | Results |
---|---|---|
Leonard et al. 2017 Noninvasive tongue stimulation combined with intensive cognitive and physical rehabilitation induces neuroplastic changes in patients with multiple sclerosis: A multimodal neuroimaging study CanadaRCT PEDro=4 NInitial=14, NFinal=14 |
Population: Active group (n=7): Mean age=47.7yr; Gender: males=3, females=4; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=4.2; Mean disease duration=11.2yr. Sham group (n=7): Mean age=49.7yr; Gender: males=3, females=4; Disease course: Unspecified; Mean EDSS=4.2; Mean disease duration=22.3yr Intervention: MS patients were randomized to receive either active non-invasive electrical tongue stimulation or sham stimulation. All subjects received intensive physical training for walking and balance (14wks) and concurrent working memory training on the computer-based memory training software program (COGMED) for 10 of the 14wks. Assessments were performed at baseline and after 14wks of treatment. Cognitive Outcome Measures: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI); California Verbal Learning Test II (CVLT-II); D-Kefs trails: color/word, verbal fluency; Tower of London 2nd edition; Ruff 2 and 7; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV): letter-number sequencing, coding, and symbol search; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); Cognitive function inventory for MS.3. |
|
Table 83. Summary Table of Studies Examining Muscle Electrical Stimulation
Executive Function | Information Processing Speed | Memory | |
Improve | |||
No statistical sig. difference |
|
|
|
Bold | RCT PEDro ≥ 6 |
Regular | RCT PEDro < 6 or PCT |
italics | Non-RCT |
Discussion
One RCT investigated the use of non-invasive cranial nerve modulation delivered via electrical tongue stimulation combined with multi-modal training on walking and balance, as well as working memory and fMRI outcomes in individuals with moderate disability (EDSS 3-6) (Leonard et al. 2017). The portable neuromodulation stimulator device (PoNSTM version 2.2) was used in both groups to either deliver the electrical stimulation (intervention group), or sham treatment (control group).
Participants in both groups in the Leonard et al. study also completed physical therapy exercises and computer cognitive training targeting memory using the COGMED software program. Targeted computer based cognitive training may improve cognition on related cognitive testing in PwMS, with possible dose training effects (see section 3.2). In a small study of PwMS, five days a week working memory training with COGMED over five weeks improved memory on a related Color Word Interference Test (Blair et al. 2021). In the Leonard et al. PoNSTM study, the COGMED protocol included training four times per week for the final 10 weeks. The physical training involved balance and walking drills, motor-control exercises, and practicing breathing and awareness techniques. The proposed physical training dose was extremely high, totaling a target of 180 minutes daily over 14 weeks. Exercise training may also influence cognitive testing results (see section 3.26). The Leonard et al. study did not report on adherence with these extensive training programs for either the control group or the intervention group. The PoNSTM device was held between the lips and teeth, and in the intervention group, stimulation over the anterior superior tongue was increased until the participants felt "moderate intense tingling" (Leonard et al. 2017, p. 3).
There were no statistically significant differences between the intervention and sham control groups for the cognitive outcomes. The study was small (n=14), not powered to detect clinically meaningful change, and the principal investigator was not blinded. Pre-post training, some of the cognitive outcomes targeting memory improved in both groups, with a trend towards larger improvements in the intervention group. Differences on fMRI during a probed memory task were reported, with changes in the left primary motor cortex seen only in the intervention group. Authors include data from a healthy control group and a "rollover" group of PwMS in describing the fMRI results, groups not mentioned in the initial methods section of the study.
The Leonard et al. (2017) study involved a multi-modal intervention; therefore, it is difficult to make any conclusions on the possible independent effects of tongue stimulation. The follow up in the Leonard et al. (2017) study is limited to immediately post-training. Authors do not report effect sizes or the raw scores on the cognitive tests. We do not know how cognitively impaired participants were at baseline or how much improvement occurred pre-post the training protocol. Overall, the addition of tongue stimulation to a multimodal intervention provided no additional benefit compared to sham tongue stimulation for cognition.
Conclusion
There is level 2 evidence that adding non-invasive cranial nerve stimulation to the tongue in addition to COGMED training and exercise may not improve memory, executive function, or information processing speed compared to no tongue stimulation and COGMED and exercise training (one randomized controlled trial; Leonard et al. 2017).
Preliminary evidence supports that non-invasive tongue stimulation may not improve memory, executive function, or information processing speed in persons with MS.
There is level 1a evidence that the following interventions may benefit cognition in MS on one or more cognitive outcomes:
There is level 1a evidence that the following interventions do not benefit cognition in MS on one or more cognitive outcomes:
There is level 1b evidence that the following interventions may benefit cognition in MS on one or more cognitive outcomes:
There is level 1b evidence that the following interventions do not benefit cognition in MS on one or more cognitive outcomes:
Level of Evidence Statements
Statements are listed from the highest to lowest levels of evidence, and in the order found in the table of contents for the categories of interventions.
Level 1a – benefits cognition in one or more cognitive domain
Computer Based Cognitive Rehabilitation Approaches
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation that targets executive function improves executive function compared to no treatment (from seven randomized controlled trials and one prospective controlled trial; De Giglio et al. 2015, De Giglio et al. 2016, Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Naeeni Davarani et al. 2020, Sharifi et al. 2019, and Tesar et al. 2005).
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation that specifically targets information processing speed does improve information processing speed in persons with MS compared to no treatment or non-specific cognitive rehabilitation (from seven randomized controlled trials, one prospective controlled trial, and four pre-post studies; Barker et al. 2019, Bonavita et al. 2015, Chiaravalloti et al. 2018, Filippi et al. 2012, Fuchs et al. 2019, Fuchs et al. 2020, Guclu Altun et al. 2015, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Messinis et al. 2017, and Messinis et al. 2020, and Rahmani et al. 2020).
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation that specifically targets memory improves memory in persons with MS compared to no treatment or their usual clinical care (from six randomized controlled trials, two prospective controlled trials, and two pre-post studies; Arian Darestani et al. 2020, Bonzano et al. 2020, Covey et al. 2018, Hildebrandt et al. 2007, Janssen et al. 2015, Mendozzi et al. 1998, Messinis et al. 2017, Messinis et al. 2020, Rahmani et al. 2020, Shatil et al. 2010, and Stuifbergen et al. 2012, and Vogt et al. 2009).
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation delivered for 33 hours or longer over at least twelve weeks improves verbal language skills compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from four randomized controlled trials; Arsoy et al. 2018, Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, and Mattioli et al. 2012).
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom modules that target executive function improves executive function for persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to no treatment (from four randomized controlled trials; Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, and Tesar et al. 2015).
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom that specifically target information processing speed does improve information processing speed in persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to no treatment or standard MS rehabilitation (from four randomized controlled trials and one prospective controlled trial; Bonavita et al. 2015, Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Messinis et al. 2017, and Messinis et al. 2020).
There is level 1a evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom delivered for 33 hours or longer over twelve weeks improves verbal language skills compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from three randomized controlled trials; Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, and Mattioli et al. 2012).
There is level 1a evidence that the modified Story Memory Technique does improve verbal learning and memory in persons with MS but may not improve other forms of memory (from five randomized controlled trials; Chiaravalloti et al. 2020, Chiaravalloti et al. 2013, Chiaravalloti et al. 2012, Dobryakova et al. 2014, Krch et al. 2019).
There is level 1a evidence that mental visual imagery training improves memory on an autobiographical memory interview assessment compared to sham verbal training or no intervention in relapsing-remitting MS. Other objective memory and cognitive outcomes are not reported (from three randomized controlled trials and two pre-post studies; Ernst et al. 2018, Ernst et al. 2016, Ernst et al. 2015, Ernst et al. 2013, and Ernst et al. 2012).
Level 1a – no objective benefit in one more cognitive domain
There is level 1a evidence that teaching the Self-Generation Technique may not significantly improve verbal memory (two randomized controlled trials; Goverover et al. 2018 and Chiaravalloti et al. 2019).
There is level 1a evidence that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may not improve auditory information processing speed in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Manglani et al. 2020, Senders et al. 2018, and Blankespoor et al. 2017).
Cognitive-Motor Dual Task Training
There is level 1a evidence that dual task training does not improve information processing speed or memory more than balance or gait training alone (from two randomized controlled trials; Sosnoff et al. 2007; Veldkamp et al. 2019).
There is level 1a evidence that cycling does not improve information processing speed compared to waitlist control in persons with MS (from three randomized controlled trials; Baquet et al. 2018, Briken et al. 2014, and Oken et al. 2004).
There is level 1a evidence that cycling does not improve executive function for persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trials; Briken et al. 2014, Oken et al. 2004).
Level 1b – benefits cognition in one or more cognitive domain
Cognitive Rehabilitation, Mixed Non-computer Approaches
There is level 1b evidence that the REACTIV program, which targets attention, may improve attention more than non-specific cognitive exercises in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from one randomized controlled trial; Lamargue et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that the french ProCog-SEP involving facilitation and reorganization training improves memory in persons with MS more than non-cognitive training and discussion (from one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Brissart et al. 2020 and Brissart et al. 2013).
There is level 1b evidence that the REACTIV protocol may improve verbal learning and memory but not other aspects of memory in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from one randomized controlled trial; Lamargue et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that compensatory strategies targeting attention and memory may improve memory more than no treatment (Mousavi et al. 2018 and Mousavi et al. 2018b).
Computer Based Cognitive Rehabilitation Approaches
Freshminder 2There is level 1b evidence that Freshminder 2 combined with counseling for compensatory strategies may improve attention in persons with MS compared to no treatment (from one randomized controlled trial; Pusswald et al. 2014).
RehaComThere is level 1b evidence that RehaCom improves attention more than computer-based visuomotor training in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from one randomized controlled trial; Cerasa et al. 2016).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using RehaCom for 60 minutes per day 2 days per week for 6 weeks may improve executive function in persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to computer-based visuomotor tasks (from one randomized controlled trial; Cerasa et al. 2013).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation with RehaCom targeting memory training may improve memory (from two randomized controlled trials and one prospective controlled trial; Arian Darestani et al. 2020, Mendozzi et al. 1998, and Messinis et al. 2017).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation targeting reaction time may improve reaction time more than computer-based cognitive rehabilitation targeting selective attention, working memory, and executive function in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Flachenecker et al. 2017).
Speed of Processing TrainingThere is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using Speed of Processing Training may improve information processing speed in persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to no treatment (from one randomized controlled trial and three pre-post study; Barker et al. 2019, Chiaravalloti et al. 2018, Fuchs et al. 2019, and Fuchs et al. 2020).
NOROSOFT Mental Exercise SoftwareThere is level 1b evidence that computer-based rehabilitation using NOROSOFT Mental Exercise Software for 24 weeks may help maintain executive function in persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to no treatment (from one randomized controlled trial; Arsoy et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using NOROSOFT delivered for 100 hours over twenty-four weeks maintains verbal language skills compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from one randomized controlled trial; Arsoy et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using VILAT-G software may improve information processing speed more than no treatment in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Hildebrandt et al. 2007).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using VILAT-G to specifically target memory may improve memory in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Hildebrandt et al. 2007).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using ERICA to specifically target memory improves spatial memory but not verbal learning and memory more than traditional cognitive rehabilitation (from one randomized controlled trial; De Luca et al. 2019).
LumosityThere is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using Lumosity to specifically target memory combined with group compensatory strategies may improve memory in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Stuifbergen et al. 2012).
There is level 1b evidence that Nintendo's Brain Training video games do improve executive function and information processing speed in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial and one randomized controlled trial with pre-post analysis; DeGiglio et al. 2015, DeGiglio et al. 2016).
There is level 1b evidence that cognitive rehabilitation in the BTS-Nirvana Virtual Reality environment may improve information processing speed and memory more than traditional cognitive rehabilitation in persons with MS (one randomized control larger trial; Maggio et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that selective reminding tasks may improve memory in persons with MS compared to single trial encoding conditions (one randomized controlled trial; McKeever et al. 2019).
There is level 1b evidence that Self-generation Technique improves contextual recall on tasks where the technique is applied compared to not applying the technique (one randomized controlled trial, one prospective controlled trial and six pre-post studies; Goverover et al. 2018; 2014; 2013; 2011; and 2008; Basso et al. 2008 and 2006; O'Brien et al. 2007; Chiaravalloti & Deluca 2002).
There is level 1b evidence that neurologic music therapy combined with cognitive rehabilitation may improve memory more than conventional cognitive rehabilitation (one randomized controlled trial; Impellizzeri et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that a walking, cycling, and ROM exercise program while wearing a cooling garment may improve verbal fluency compared to the same exercise program without a cooling garment in persons with MS (one randomized controlled trial; Gonzales et al. 2017).
There is level 1b evidence that cycling may improve memory for patients with progressive MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Briken et al. 2014).
High Intensity Interval Training
There is level 1b evidence that high intensity aerobic cycling training may improve verbal memory compared to moderate intensity aerobic cycling training in people with a baseline VO2 peak of ~20mL/kg/min (one randomized controlled trial; Zimmer et al. 2018).
Balance Training and Dual Task
There is level 1b evidence that balance training combined with a dual task may improve general cognitive impairment and executive function at 6 months compared to no intervention (one randomized controlled trial; Felippe et al. 2019).
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation
There is level 1b evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation combined with cognitive training may improve executive function compared to sham treatment combined with cognitive training (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Chalah et al. 2017, Charvet et al. 2018).
Level 1b – no objective benefit in one or more cognitive domain
Cognitive Rehabilitation, Mixed Non-computer Approaches
There is level 1b evidence that memory and attention rehabilitation using education and compensatory strategies may not improve attention more than sham psychoeducation in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from one randomized controlled trial; Mani et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that compensatory memory strategies may not improve memory more than restitution in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from one randomized controlled trial; Martin et al. 2014).
There is level 1b evidence that the french ProCog-SEP program involving facilitation and reorganization training may not improve information processing speed more than non-cognitive exercises and discussion (from one randomized controlled trial, Brissart et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that the REACTIV program may not improve information processing more than non-specific cognitive training and physical activity (from one randomized controlled trial; Lamargue et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that group cognitive training that focuses on compensatory strategies and restitution for memory and attention may not improve information processing speed compared to usual care (defined as advice from nursing and OT) in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Lincoln et al. 2020).
Computer-Based Cognitive Rehabiliatiation Approaches
VILAT-GThere is level 1b evidence that VILAT-G may not improve attention in persons with MS compared to no treatment (from one randomized controlled trial; Shatil et al. 2010).
Attention-specific trainingThere is level 1b evidence that computer-based Attention Processing Training (APT) may not improve all attention domains compared to non-specific cognitive exercises in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Amato et al. 2014).
MAPSS-MS (Lumosity + neuropsychonline + group therapy for compensatory strategies)There is level 1b evidence that the MAPSS-MS program, which combines Lumosity for 45 minutes per day 3 times per week for 8 weeks with group therapy for compensatory strategies, may not improve executive function compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from one randomized controlled trial; Stuifbergen et al. 2012).
LumosityThere is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using Lumosity in the MAPSS-MS program may not improve information processing speed compared to no treatment in persons with MS with cognitively impaired (from one randomized controlled trial; Stuifbergen et al. 2012).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using Lumosity to specifically target memory combined with group compensatory strategies may not improve more than MyBrainGames on multiplesclerosis.com (from one randomized controlled trial; Stuifbergen et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using NOROSOFT may not improve memory in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Arsoy et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that robot-assisted gait training in a virtual reality environment may not improve information processing speed, memory, or verbal language skills more than robot-assisted gait training (one randomized controlled small trial; Munari et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that strobic visual training may improve information processing speed but not memory, executive function, attention, verbal function, or global cognitive scores (one crossover RCT study; Shalmoni et al., 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that robotics may not improve any measure of cognition more than gait-training in persons with MS (from one very small randomized controlled trial; Munari et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that teaching the Self-Generation Technique may not significantly improve visuospatial memory (one randomized controlled trial; Chiaravalloti et. al. 2019).
There is level 1b evidence that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may improve visual information processing speed in persons with MS (from one random trial; Manglani et al., 2020).
Social Cognitive Theory Education
There is level 1b evidence that social cognitive education combined with aerobic and strength exercise may not improve information processing more than attention control education combined with aerobic and strength exercise (from one randomized controlled trial; Coote et al. 2017).
Cognitive-Motor Dual Task Training
There is level 1b evidence that dual task training does not improve attention more than gait training alone (from one randomized controlled trial; Veldkamp et al. 2019).
Cognitive-Motor Dual Task Training
There is level 1b evidence that dual task training does not improve executive function more than strength training (from one randomized controlled trial; Jonsdottir et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that aerobic and strength training combined may not improve information processing speed in persons with MS at six months follow up (from two randomized controlled trials; Coghe et al. 2018. Sandroff et al. 2017).
There is level 1b evidence that aerobic and strength training combined may not improve attention in persons with relapsing-remitting MS at six months follow up (from one randomized controlled trial; Coghe et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that cycling may not improve memory for persons with relapsing-remitting MS (from one randomized controlled trial and two pre-post studies; Baquet et al. 2018, Barry et al. 2018, and Swank et al. 2013).
There is level 1b evidence that cycling may not improve verbal fluency for persons with progressive MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Briken et al. 2014).
High Intensity Interval Training
There is level 1b evidence that high intensity aerobic cycling training may not improve attention, processing speed or visual spatial memory compared to moderate intensity aerobic cycling training in people with a baseline V02 peak of ~20mL/kg/min (one randomized controlled trial; Zimmer et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that circuit training may not improve memory, verbal fluency, visual processing speed, or auditory processing speed significantly more than relaxation exercises (one randomized controlled trial, Ozkul et al. 2020).
There is level 1b evidence that walking programs may not improve verbal learning and memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Sandroff et al. 2017).
There is level 1b evidence that square stepping may not improve visual spatial memory, verbal memory, or visual processing speed significantly more than a light stretching and strengthening program (one randomized controlled trial; Sebastião et al. 2018).
There is level 1b evidence that a stepping exergame program may not improve visual spatial processing speed significantly more than usual physical activities (one randomized controlled trial; Hoang et al. 2016).
High Frequency Repetitive Transcanial Magnetic Stimulation
There is level 1b evidence that high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) may not significantly improve working memory compared to sham rTMS in persons with MS without cognitive impairment at baseline (from one randomized controlled trial; Hulst et al. 2017).
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation
There is level 1b evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation may not improve basic attention compared to sham treatment (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Chalah et al. 2017, Charvet et al. 2018).
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation
There is level 1b evidence that transcranial direct current stimulation may not improve memory in persons with MS compared to sham treatment (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Mattioli et al. 2016; Charvet et al. 2018).
Transcranial Random Noise Stimulation
There is level 1b evidence that transcranial random noise stimulation may not improve attention in persons with MS compared to placebo (from one randomized controlled trial; Palm et al. 2016).
Level 2 – benefits cognition in one or more cognitive domain
Cognitive Rehabilitation, Mixed Non-computer Approaches
There is level 2 evidence that n-back training over the course of 1 week may improve working memory compared to no treatment (from one randomized controlled trial; Aguirre et al. 2019).
There is level 2 evidence that the REHACOP protocol might improve memory compared to no treatment (from one randomized controlled trial; Rilo et al. 2013).
There is level 2 evidence that the REHACOP protocol may improve executive function more than no treatment for persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Rilo et al. 2018).
There is level 2 evidence that executive functioning training using executive function textbook exercises may improve executive function more than RehaCom reaction time training or no treatment (from one prospective controlled trial; Fink et al. 2010).
There is level 2 evidence that the REHACOP protocol may improve information processing more than no treatment in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Rilo et al. 2018).
There is level 2 evidence that Tele-MIT may improve information processing more than no treatment in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Kahraman et al. 2020).
Computer Based Cognitive Rehabilitation Approaches
ERICAThere is level 2 evidence that ERICA attention exercises may improve attention in persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to pen-and-paper attention exercises (from one randomized controlled trial; Orel et al. 2014).
CogniFitThere is level 2 evidence that computer-based cognitive rehabilitation using CogniFit to specifically target memory may improve memory in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Hildebrandt et al. 2007).
There is level 2 evidence that the Space Fortress video game may improve spatial and visuospatial memory in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Janssen et al. 2015).
There is level 2 evidence that spaced learning improves memory compared to mass learning (one prospective controlled trial and two pre-post studies; Goverover et al. 2009, Sumowski et al. 2010 and Sumowski et al. 2013).
There is level 2 evidence that retrieval practice improves memory in persons with MS with mild or advanced cognitive impairment to a greater extent then spaced learning or mass learning approaches (two pre-post studies; Sumowski et al. 2010 and Sumowski et al. 2013).
There is level 2 evidence that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may improve attention and verbal skills in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial reporting pre-post results; De la Torre et al. 2020).
There is level 2 evidence that meditation may improve information processing speed in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (from one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Bhargav et al., 2016, Anagnostouli et al., 2019).
There is level 2 evidence that eight weeks of cognitive behavioural therapy or dialectical behavioural therapy may improve memory in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Abdolghaddri et al. 2019).
Social Cognitive Theory Education
There is level 2 evidence that social cognitive education combined with aerobic and strength exercise may improve information processing speed (within group pre-post results from one randomized controlled trial; Coote et al. 2017).
There is level 2 evidence that running may improve spatial memory but not verbal learning and memory or information processing speed in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled studies; Feyst et al. 2019; Huiskamp et al. 2020).
There is level 2 evidence that Pilates may improve information processing speed compared to traditional exercise programs in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Abasiyanik et al. 2020, Küçük et al. 2016, and Kara et al. 2017).
There is level 2 evidence that Pilates may improve memory in persons with MS compared to traditional exercise programs (from one randomized controlled trial; Abasiyanik et al. 2020).
There is level 2 evidence that yoga may improve attention more than sports climbing in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Velikonja et al. 2010).
Functional Electrical Stimulation Cycling
There is level 2 evidence that functional electrical stimulation cycling may improve visual processing speed compared to passive cycling in persons with MS with mobility impairments (from one randomized controlled trial; Pilutti et al., 2019).
Level 2 – no objective benefit in one or more cognitive outcomes
Cognitive Rehabilitation, Mixed Non-computer Approaches
There is level 2 evidence that the REHACOP protocol may not improve attention more than no treatment for persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Riloet al. 2018).
There is level 2 evidence that cognitive rehabilitation targeting executive function may not improve executive function more than normal MS rehab and physiotherapy (from one randomized controlled trial; Hanssen et al. 2016).
Computer Based Cognitive Rehabilitation Approaches
CogniFit 2There is level 2 evidence that CogniFit 2 may not improve attention in persons with MS compared to no treatment (from one randomized controlled trial; Shatil et al. 2010).
Captain's LogThere is level 2 evidence that computer-based rehabilitation using Captain's Log software for 6 weeks may improve executive function in persons with MS compared to no treatment (from one prospective controlled trial; Sharifi et al. 2019).
There is level 2 evidence that the Space Fortress video game may not improve verbal learning and memory in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Janssen et al. 2015).
There is level 2 evidence that meditation may not improve executive function in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Bhargav et al., 2016).
There is level 2 evidence that eight weeks of cognitive behavioural therapy or dialectical behavioural therapy may not improve attention in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial; Abdolghaddri et al. 2019).
There is level 2 evidence that adding non-invasive cranial nerve stimulation to the tongue in addition to COGMED training and exercise may not improve memory, executive function, or information processing speed compared to no tongue stimulation and COGMED and exercise training (from one randomized controlled trial; Leonard et al. 2017).
Level 4 – benefits cognition in one or more cognitive domain
Cognitive Rehabilitation, Mixed Non-computer Approaches
There is level 4 evidence that practicing mental imagery with mnemonic memory techniques together may improve prospective memory when playing a board game in minimally cognitive impaired persons with MS more than in healthy controls (from one pre-post study; Kardiasmenos et al. 2008).
There is level 4 evidence that EEG neurofeedback training may improve long-term memory and executive function (from one pre-post study; Kober et al., 2019).
There is level 4 evidence that cue salience may improve prospective memory in both high- and low-executive functioning persons with MS (one pre-post trial; Dagenais et al. 2016).
There is level 4 evidence that group psychotherapy may improve auditory information processing speed but not visual information processing speed in persons with MS who have depression and CI (from one pre-post trial; Bilgi et al. 2015).
There is level 4 evidence that watching daily life hand movements (action observation) may improve auditory processing speed in persons with MS receiving an upper limb rehabilitation program (from pre-post data in one randomized controlled study; Rocca et al., 2019).
There is level 4 evidence that team-based artistic therapy, consisting of photography, painting, poetry, and videography, may improve visual information processing speed and memory but not auditory information processing speed in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (one pre-post study; Van Geel et al. 2020).
There is level 4 evidence that a modified paleolithic diet combined with electrical stimulation, exercise, and stress management may improve executive functioning (one pre-post study; Lee et al. 2017).
Level 4 – no objective benefit in one or more cognitive domains
There is level 4 evidence that mindfulness-based cognitive therapies may not improve executive function in persons with MS (from one pre-post study; Blankespoor et al. 2017).
There is level 4 evidence that a Cognitive Occupation-Based Program may not improve processing speed or executive function, however ADLs and IADLs and occupational competence may improve by self-report (one pre-post study; Reilly et al., 2018).
There is level 4 evidence that memory is worse when the body temperature is lowered by one degree Celsius compared to a resting control temperature in persons with MS (from one pre-post study; Geisler et al. 1996).
Cognitive Rehabilitation, Mixed Non-computer Approaches
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves attention in persons with MS (from three randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Brenk et al. 2008, Lamargue et al. 2020, Mani et al. 2018, and Rilo et al. 2018).
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves memory in persons with MS among studies with different rehabilitation interventions, comparator groups and memory outcomes (from twelve randomized controlled trials, four prospective controlled trials, and one pre-post study; Aguirre et al. 2019, Brenk et al. 2008, Brissart et al. 2013, Brissart et al. 2020, Carr et al. 2014, Fink et al. 2010, Goverover et al. 2009, Jonsson et al. 1993, Kahraman et al. 2020, Lamargue et al. 2020, Lincoln et al. 2020, Mani et al. 2018, Martin et al. 2014, Mousavi et al. 2018, , Rilo et al. 2018, Rodgers et al., 1996, and Shahpouri et al. 2019).
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves executive function in persons with MS (from four randomized controlled trials, one prospective controlled trial, and one pre-post study; Brenk et al. 2008, Fink et al. 2010, Hanssen et al. 2016, Lincoln et al. 2002, Mani et al. 2018, and Rilo et al., 2018).
There is conflicting evidence whether memory and attention cognitive rehabilitation combined with compensatory strategies improves executive function in person with MS (from two randomized controlled trials; Lincoln et al. 2002 and Mani et al. 2018).
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves information processing speed in persons with MS (from five randomized controlled trials, one prospective trial study, and one pre-post study; Brissart et al. 2020, Kahraman et al. 2020, Lamargue et al. 2020, Lincoln et al. 2020, Rilo et al. 2018, Rodgers et al., 1996, and Zuber et al. 2020).
There is conflicting evidence whether cognitive rehabilitation improves verbal language skills in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Brissart et al. 2013 and Rilo et al. 2013).
Computer Based Cognitive Rehabilitation Approaches
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation delivered for less than 33 hours of total training is more effective than no treatment (from four randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Barker et al. 2019, Stuifbergen et al. 2012, Mäntynen et al. 2014, Arian Darestani et al. 2020, and Pusswald et al. 2014).
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves attention in persons with MS (from seventeen randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Amato et al. 2014, Campbell et al. 2016, Cerasa et al. 2013, Filippi et al. 2012, Flachenecker et al. 2017, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Messinis et al. 2017, Orel 2014, Plohmann et al. 1998, Rahmani et al. 2020, and Tesar et al. 2005).
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves executive function in persons with MS (from sixteen randomized controlled trials and two prospective controlled trials; Amato et al. 2014, Arsoy et al. 2018, Bonavita et al. 2015, Cerasa et al. 2013, De Giglio et al. 2015, De Giglio et al. 2016, Filippi et al. 2012, Grasso et al. 2017, Hancock et al. 2015, Mäntynen et al. 2014, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Naeeni Davarani et al. 2020, Rahmani et al. 2020, Sharifi et al. 2019, Stuifbergen et al. 2012, and Tesar et al. 2005).
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves memory in persons with MS (from 18 randomized controlled trials, 6 prospective controlled trials and 1 pre-post study; Amato et al. 2014, Arian Darestani et al. 2020, Arsoy et al. 2018, Barker et al. 2019, Bonavita et al. 2015, Bonzano et al. 2020, Bove et al. 2021, Campbell et al. 2016, Cerasa et al. 2013, Chiaravalloti et al. 2018, Covey et al. 2018, De Luca et al. 2019, Filippi et al. 2012, Fuchs et al. 2019, Hildebrandt et al. 2007, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Mendozzi et al. 1998, Messinis et al. 2017, Messinis et al. 2020, Shatil et al. 2010, Solari et al. 2004, Vogt et al. 2009, and Allen et al., 2018).
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation improves verbal language skills in persons with MS (from nine randomized controlled trials; Arian Darestani et al. 2020, Arsoy et al. 2018, Filippi et al. 2012, Mäntynen et al. 2014, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Pusswald et al. 2014, Stuifbergen et al. 2012, and Stuifbergen et al. 2018).
There is conflicting evidence whether RehaCom improves attention in persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to no treatment (from six randomized controlled trials and one prospective controlled trial; Filippi et al. 2012, Mattioli et al. 2010, Mattioli et al. 2012, Mendozzi et al. 1998, Messinis et al. 2017, Naeeni Davarni et al. 2020, and Tesar et al. 2005).
There is conflicting evidence whether using RehaCom for 8 weeks or less improves executive function for persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to no treatment or non-specific treatment (from one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Bonavita et al. 2015 and Tesar et al. 2015).
There is conflicting evidence whether computer-based cognitive rehabilitation with RehaCom targeting memory training improves memory in persons with MS with cognitively impaired compared to natural history DVDs or nonspecific computer exercises (from two randomized controlled trials; Campbell et al. 2016 and Messinis et al. 2020).
There is conflicting evidence whether mindfulness-based cognitive therapies improve memory in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trials and one pre-post study; Blankespoor et al. 2017, De la Torre et al. 2020, and Manglani et al. 2020).
There is conflicting evidence whether meditation may improve memory in persons with relapsing-remitting MS (from one randomized controlled trial and one prospective controlled trial; Bhargav et al., 2016, Anagnostouli et al., 2019).
There is conflicting evidence whether music mnemonics improve memory compared to spoken words in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trials; Moore et al. 2008, Thaut et al. 2014).
There is conflicting evidence whether cooling may improve information processing in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trials; Gonzales et al. 2017; Schwid et al. 2013).
There is conflicting evidence whether cycling improves attention in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trials; Briken et al. 2014 and Oken et al. 2004).
There is conflicting evidence whether dance training improves information processing speed in persons with MS (two pre-post studies; Van Geel et al. 2020; Ng et al. 2019).
There is conflicting evidence whether walking programs may improve information processing speed in persons with MS (from one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Sandroff et al. 2016 and Van Geel et al. 2020).
There is conflicting evidence whether walking programs improve executive function in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trial; Sandroff et al. 2016 and Sandroff et al. 2015).
There is conflicting evidence whether yoga improves executive function in persons with MS (from two randomized controlled trials; Oken et al. 2004, Velikonja et al. 2010).
Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation
There is conflicting evidence whether transcranial direct current stimulation combined with cognitive training improves visual information processing compared to sham treatment combined with cognitive training (one randomized controlled trial and one pre-post study; Mattioli et al. 2016, Charvet et al. 2018).
Knowledge
Innovation
Talent
Everywhere
K . I . T . E
KITE at UHN